Customer experience: a systematic literature review and consumer culture theory-based conceptualisation

  • Published: 15 February 2020
  • Volume 71 , pages 135–176, ( 2021 )

Cite this article

  • Muhammad Waqas 1 ,
  • Zalfa Laili Binti Hamzah 1 &
  • Noor Akma Mohd Salleh 2  

10k Accesses

45 Citations

2 Altmetric

Explore all metrics

The study aims to summarise and classify the existing research and to better understand the past, present, and the future state of the theory of customer experience. The main objectives of this study are to categorise and summarise the customer experience research, identify the extant theoretical perspectives that are used to conceptualise the customer experience, present a new conceptualisation and conceptual model of customer experience based on consumer culture theory and to highlight the emerging trends and gaps in the literature of customer experience. To achieve the stated objectives, an extensive literature review of existing customer experience research was carried out covering 49 journals. A total of 99 empirical and conceptual articles on customer experience from the year 1998 to 2019 was analysed based on different criteria. The findings of this study contribute to the knowledge by highlighting the role of customer attribution of meanings in defining their experiences and how such experiences can predict consumer behaviour.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

customer feedback research paper

Similar content being viewed by others

customer feedback research paper

Customer experience: fundamental premises and implications for research

Larissa Becker & Elina Jaakkola

customer feedback research paper

Components of Customer Experience and their Impact on Co-created Value: An Abstract

Customer experience management: toward implementing an evolving marketing concept.

Christian Homburg, Danijel Jozić & Christina Kuehnl

Abbott L (1955) Quality and competition. Columbia University Press, New York

Google Scholar  

Addis M, Holbrook MB (2001) On the conceptual link between mass customisation and experiential consumption: an explosion of subjectivity. J Consum Behav 1:50–66

Alben L (1996) Defining the criteria for effective interaction design. Interactions 3:11–15

Allen CT, Fournier S, Miller F (2008) Brands and their meaning makers. In: Haugtvedt C, Herr P, Kardes F (eds) Handbook of consumer psychology. Taylor & Francis, Milton Park, pp 781–822

Andreini D, Pedeliento G, Zarantonello L, Solerio C (2019) A renaissance of brand experience: advancing the concept through a multi-perspective analysis. J Bus Res 91:123–133

Arnold MJ, Reynolds KE, Ponder N, Lueg JE (2005) Customer delight in a retail context: investigating delightful and terrible shopping experiences. J Bus Res 58:1132–1145

Arnould EJ, Thompson CJ (2005) Consumer culture theory (CCT): twenty years of research. J Consum Res 31:868–882

Beckman E, Kumar A, Kim Y-K (2013) The impact of brand experience on downtown success. J Travel Res 52:646–658

Belk RW, Costa JA (1998) The mountain man myth: a contemporary consuming fantasy. J Consum Res 25:218–240

Bennett R, Härtel CE, McColl-Kennedy JR (2005) Experience as a moderator of involvement and satisfaction on brand loyalty in a business-to-business setting 02-314R. Ind Mark Manag 34:97–107

Berry LL (2000) Cultivating service brand equity. J Acad Mark Sci 28:128–137

Berry LL, Carbone LP, Haeckel SH (2002) Managing the total customer experience. MIT Sloan Manag Rev 43:85–89

Biedenbach G, Marell A (2010) The impact of customer experience on brand equity in a business-to-business services setting. J Brand Manag 17:446–458

Bilgihan A, Okumus F, Nusair K, Bujisic M (2014) Online experiences: flow theory, measuring online customer experience in e-commerce and managerial implications for the lodging industry. Inf Technol Tour 14:49–71

Bilro RG, Loureiro SMC, Ali F (2018) The role of website stimuli of experience on engagement and brand advocacy. J Hosp Tour Technol 9:204–222

Bolton RN, Kannan PK, Bramlett MD (2000) Implications of loyalty program membership and service experiences for customer retention and value. J Acad Mark Sci 28:95–108

Brakus JJ, Schmitt BH, Zarantonello L (2009) Brand experience: what is it? How is it measured? Does it affect loyalty? J Mark 73:52–68

Branch JD (2007) Postmodern consumption and the high-fidelity audio microculture. In: Belk RW, Sherry JF Jr (eds) Consumer culture theory. JAI Press, Oxford

Braun-LaTour KA, LaTour MS (2005) Transforming consumer experience: when timing matters. J Advert 34:19–30

Braun-LaTour KA, LaTour MS, Pickrell JE, Loftus EF, SUia (2004) How and when advertising can influence memory for consumer experience. J Advert 33:7–25

Bridges E, Florsheim R (2008) Hedonic and utilitarian shopping goals: the online experience. J Bus Res 61:309–314

Brodie RJ, Ilic A, Juric B, Hollebeek L (2013) Consumer engagement in a virtual brand community: an exploratory analysis. J Bus Res 66:105–114

Bronner F, Neijens P (2006) Audience experiences of media context and embedded advertising-a comparison of eight media international. Int J Mark Res 48:81–100

Calder BJ, Malthouse EC, Schaedel U (2009) An experimental study of the relationship between online engagement and advertising effectiveness. J Interact Mark 23:321–331

Carù A, Cova B (2003) Revisiting consumption experience: a more humble but complete view of the concept. Mark Lett 3:267–286

Chandler JD, Lusch RF (2015) Service systems: a broadened framework and research agenda on value propositions, engagement, and service experience. J Serv Res 18:6–22

Constantinides E (2004) Influencing the online consumer’s behavior: the web experience. Internet Res 14:111–126

Constantinides E, Lorenzo-Romero C, Gómez MA (2010) Effects of web experience on consumer choice: a multicultural approach. Internet Res 20:188–209

Cooper H, Schembri S, Miller D (2010) Brand-self identity narratives in the James Bond movies. Psychol Mark 27:557–567

Cova B (1997) Community and consumption: towards a definition of the “linking value” of product or services. Eur J Mark 31:297–316

Creswell JW (2007) Qualitative inquiry and research design: choosing among five approaches, 2nd edn. Sage Publications, Thousand Oaks

Das K (2009) Relationship marketing research (1994–2006) an academic literature review and classification. Mark Intell Plan 27:326–363

Das G, Agarwal J, Malhotra NK, Varshneya G (2019) Does brand experience translate into brand commitment? A mediated-moderation model of brand passion and perceived brand ethicality. J Bus Res 95:479–490

Daugherty T, Li H, Biocca F (2008) Consumer learning and the effects of virtual experience relative to indirect and direct product experience. Psychol Mark 25:568–586

De Keyser A, Lemon KN, Klaus P, Keiningham TL (2015) A framework for understanding and managing the customer experience. Marketing Science Institute working paper series, pp 15–121

de Oliveira Santini F, Ladeira WJ, Sampaio CH, Pinto DC (2018) The brand experience extended model: a meta-analysis. J Brand Manag 25:519–535

De Vries L, Gensler S, Leeflang PS (2012) Popularity of brand posts on brand fan pages: an investigation of the effects of social media marketing. J Interact Mark 26:83–91

Ding CG, Tseng TH (2015) On the relationships among brand experience, hedonic emotions, and brand equity. Eur J Mark 49:994–1015

Elliot S, Fowell S (2000) Expectations versus reality: a snapshot of consumer experiences with internet retailing. Int J Inf Manag 20:323–336

Escalas et al (2013) Self-identity and consumer behavior. J Consum Res 39:15–18

Farndale E, Kelliher C (2013) Implementing performance appraisal: exploring the employee experience. Hum Resour Manag 52:879–897

Fisch C, Block J (2018) Six tips for your (systematic) literature review in business and management research. Manag Rev Q 68:103–106

Flavián C, Guinalíu M, Gurrea R (2006) The influence of familiarity and usability on loyalty to online journalistic services: the role of user experience. J Retail Consum Serv 13:363–375

Forlizzi J, Battarbee K (2004) Understanding experience in interactive systems. In: Proceedings of the 5th conference on designing interactive systems: processes, practices, methods, and techniques. ACM, New York, pp 261–268

Frambach RT, Roest HC, Krishnan TV (2007) The impact of consumer internet experience on channel preference and usage intentions across the different stages of the buying process. J Interact Mark 21:26–41

Froehle CM, Roth AV (2004) New measurement scales for evaluating perceptions of the technology-mediated customer service experience. J Oper Manag 22:1–21

Frow P, Payne A (2007) Towards the ‘perfect’ customer experience. J Brand Manag 15:89–101

Garg R, Rahman Z, Kumar I (2011) Customer experience: a critical literature review and research agenda. Int J Serv Sci 4:146–173

Geertz C (2008) Local knowledge: further essays in interpretive anthropology. Basic books, New York

Gensler S, Völckner F, Liu-Thompkins Y, Wiertz C (2013) Managing brands in the social media environment. J Interact Mark 27:242–256

Gentile C, Spiller N, Noci G (2007) How to sustain the customer experience: an overview of experience components that co-create value with the customer. Eur Manag J 25:395–410

Goldstein SM, Johnston R, Duffy J, Rao J (2002) The service concept: the missing link in service design research? J Oper Manag 20:121–134

Grace D, O’Cass A (2004) Examining service experiences and post-consumption evaluations. J Serv Mark 18:450–461

Greenwell TC, Fink JS, Pastore DL (2002) Assessing the influence of the physical sports facility on customer satisfaction within the context of the service experience. Sport Manag Rev 5:129–148

Grewal D, Levy M, Kumar V (2009) Customer experience management in retailing: an organizing framework. J Retail 85:1–14

Ha HY, Perks H (2005) Effects of consumer perceptions of brand experience on the web: brand familiarity, satisfaction and brand trust. J Consum Behav An Int Res Rev 4:438–452

Hamzah ZL, Alwi SFS, Othman MN (2014) Designing corporate brand experience in an online context: a qualitative insight. J Bus Res 67:2299–2310

Harris P (2007) We the people: the importance of employees in the process of building customer experience. J Brand Manag 15:102–114

Harris K, Baron S, Parker C (2000) Understanding the consumer experience: it’s’ good to talk’. J Mark Manag 16:111–127

Hassenzahl M (2008) User experience (UX): towards an experiential perspective on product quality. In: Proceedings of the 20th conference on l’Interaction Homme-Machine. ACM, pp 11–15

Hatcher EP (1999) Art as culture: an introduction to the anthropology of art. Greenwood Publishing Group, Westport

Hepola J, Karjaluoto H, Hintikka A (2017) The effect of sensory brand experience and involvement on brand equity directly and indirectly through consumer brand engagement. J Prod Brand Manag 26:282–293

Hirschman EC, Holbrook MB (1982) Hedonic consumption: emerging concepts, methods and propositions. J Mark 46:92–101

Hoffman DL, Novak TP (1996) Marketing in hypermedia computer-mediated environments: conceptual foundations. J Mark 60:50–68

Hoffman DL, Novak TP (2017) Consumer and object experience in the internet of things: an assemblage theory approach. J Consum Res 44:1178–1204

Holbrook MB, Hirschman EC (1982) The experiential aspects of consumption: consumer fantasies, feelings, and fun. J Consum Res 9:132–140

Hollebeek LD, Glynn MS, Brodie RJ (2014) Consumer brand engagement in social media: conceptualization, scale development and validation. J Int Mark 28:149–165

Holt DB (2003) Brands and branding. Harvard Business School, Boston

Homburg C, Jozić D, Kuehnl C (2017) Customer experience management: toward implementing an evolving marketing concept. J Acad Mark Sci 45:377–401

Hsu HY, Tsou H-T (2011) Understanding customer experiences in online blog environments. Int J Inf Manag 31:510–523

Huang P, Lurie NH, Mitra S (2009) Searching for experience on the web: an empirical examination of consumer behavior for search and experience goods. J Mark 73:55–69

Hultén B (2011) Sensory marketing: the multi-sensory brand-experience concept. Eur Bus Rev 23:256–273

Iglesias O, Singh JJ, Batista-Foguet JM (2011) The role of brand experience and affective commitment in determining brand loyalty. J Brand Manag 18:570–582

Islam JU, Rahman Z (2016) The transpiring journey of customer engagement research in marketing: a systematic review of the past decade. Manag Dec 54:2008–2034

Jacoby J (2002) Stimulus-organism-response reconsidered: an evolutionary step in modeling (consumer) behavior. J Consum Psychol 12:51–57

Kaplan S (1992) The restorative environment: nature and human experience. In: Relf D (ed) The role of horticulture in human well-being and social development. Timber Press, Arlington, pp 134–142

Keng C-J, Ting H-Y, Chen Y-T (2011) Effects of virtual-experience combinations on consumer-related “sense of virtual community”. Internet Res 21:408–434

Khalifa M, Liu V (2007) Online consumer retention: contingent effects of online shopping habit and online shopping experience. Eur J Inf Syst 16:780–792

Khan I, Fatma M (2017) Antecedents and outcomes of brand experience: an empirical study. J Brand Manag 24:439–452

Khan I, Rahman Z (2015) Brand experience formation mechanism and its possible outcomes: a theoretical framework. Mark Rev 15:239–259

Khan I, Rahman Z, Fatma M (2016) The role of customer brand engagement and brand experience in online banking. Int J Bank Mark 34:1025–1041

Kim H, Suh K-S, Lee U-K (2013) Effects of collaborative online shopping on shopping experience through social and relational perspectives. Inf Manag 50:169–180

Kinard BR, Hartman KB (2013) Are you entertained? The impact of brand integration and brand experience in television-related advergames. J Advert 42:196–203

Kozinets RV (2001) Utopian enterprise: articulating the meanings of Star Trek’s culture of consumption. J Consum Res 28:67–88

Kozinets RV (2002) Can consumers escape the market? Emancipatory illuminations from burning man. J Consum Res 29:20–38

Kuksov D, Shachar R, Wang K (2013) Advertising and consumers’ communications. Mark Sci 32:294–309

LaSalle D, Britton TA (2003) Priceless: turning ordinary products into extraordinary experiences. Harvard Business School Press, Boston

Laugwitz B, Held T, Schrepp M (2008) Construction and evaluation of a user experience questionnaire. In: Symposium of the Austrian HCI and usability engineering group. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, pp 63–76

Lee YH, Lim EAC (2008) What’s funny and what’s not: the moderating role of cultural orientation in ad humor. J Advert 37:71–84

Lemke F, Clark M, Wilson H (2011) Customer experience quality: an exploration in business and consumer contexts using repertory grid technique. J Acad Mark Sci 39:846–869

Lemon KN, Verhoef PC (2016) Understanding customer experience throughout the customer journey. J Mark 80:69–96

Lin YH (2015) Innovative brand experience’s influence on brand equity and brand satisfaction. J Bus Res 68:2254–2259

Lindsey-Mullikin J, Munger JL (2011) Companion shoppers and the consumer shopping experience. J Relatsh Mark 10:7–27

Loureiro SMC, de Araújo CMB (2014) Luxury values and experience as drivers for consumers to recommend and pay more. J Retail Consum Serv 21:394–400

Lundqvist A, Liljander V, Gummerus J, Van Riel A (2013) The impact of storytelling on the consumer brand experience: the case of a firm-originated story. J Brand Manag 20:283–297

Mann SJ (2001) Alternative perspectives on the student experience: alienation and engagement. Stud High Educ 26:7–19

Martin J, Mortimer G, Andrews L (2015) Re-examining online customer experience to include purchase frequency and perceived risk. J Retail Consum Serv 25:81–95

Mascarenhas OA, Kesavan R, Bernacchi M (2006) Lasting customer loyalty: a total customer experience approach. J Consum Mark 23:397–405

McCarthy J, Wright P (2004) Technology as experience. Interactions 11:42–43

Menon S, Kahn B (2002) Cross-category effects of induced arousal and pleasure on the Internet shopping experience. J Retail 78:31–40

Mersey RD, Malthouse EC, Calder BJ (2010) Engagement with online media. J Media Bus Stud 7:39–56

Meyer C, Schwager A (2007) Customer experience. Harv Bus Rev 85:1–11

Milligan A, Smith S (2002) Uncommon practice: People who deliver a great brand experience. Financial Times/Prentice Hall, London

Mollen A, Wilson H (2010) Engagement, telepresence and interactivity in online consumer experience: reconciling scholastic and managerial perspectives. J Bus Res 63:919–925

Morgan-Thomas A, Veloutsou C (2013) Beyond technology acceptance: brand relationships and online brand experience. J Bus Res 66:21–27

Mosley RW (2007) Customer experience, organisational culture and the employer brand. J Brand Manag 15:123–134

Mosteller J, Donthu N, Eroglu S (2014) The fluent online shopping experience. J Bus Res 67:2486–2493

MSI (2016) Research priorities 2016–2018. http://www.msi.org/research/2016-2018-research-priorities/ . Accessed 28 Aug 2017

MSI (2018) Research priorities 2018–2020. Marketing Science Institute, Cambridge

Mulet-Forteza C, Genovart-Balaguer J, Mauleon-Mendez E, Merigó JM (2019) A bibliometric research in the tourism, leisure and hospitality fields. J Bus Res 101:819–827

Nairn A, Griffin C, Gaya Wicks P (2008) Children’s use of brand symbolism: a consumer culture theory approach. Eur J Mark 42:627–640

Nambisan S, Baron RA (2007) Interactions in virtual customer environments: implications for product support and customer relationship management. J Int Mark 21:42–62

Ngo LV, Northey G, Duffy S, Thao HTP (2016) Perceptions of others, mindfulness, and brand experience in retail service setting. J Retail Consum Serv 33:43–52

Novak TP, Hoffman DL, Yung Y-F (2000) Measuring the customer experience in online environments: a structural modeling approach. Mark Sci 19:22–42

Nysveen H, Pedersen PE (2004) An exploratory study of customers’ perception of company web sites offering various interactive applications: moderating effects of customers’ Internet experience. Dec Support Syst 37:137–150

Nysveen H, Pedersen PE (2014) Influences of cocreation on brand experience. Int J Mark Res 56:807–832

Nysveen H, Pedersen PE, Skard S (2013) Brand experiences in service organizations: exploring the individual effects of brand experience dimensions. J Brand Manag 20:404–423

O’Cass A, Grace D (2004) Exploring consumer experiences with a service brand. J Prod Brand Manag 13:257–268

Ofir C, Raghubir P, Brosh G, Monroe KB, Heiman A (2008) Memory-based store price judgments: the role of knowledge and shopping experience. J Retail 84:414–423

Palmer A (2010) Customer experience management: a critical review of an emerging idea. J Serv Mark 24:196–208

Piedmont RL, Leach MM (2002) Cross-cultural generalizability of the spiritual transcendence scale in india: spirituality as a universal aspect of human experience. Am Behav Sci 45:1888–1901

Pine BJ, Gilmore JH (1998) Welcome to the experience economy. Harv Bus Rev 76:97–105

Ponsonby-Mccabe S, Boyle E (2006) Understanding brands as experiential spaces: axiological implications for marketing strategists. J Strateg Mark 14:175–189

Prahalad CK, Ramaswamy V (2004) Co-creation experiences: the next practice in value creation. J Interact Mark 18:5–14

Rageh Ismail A, Melewar T, Lim L, Woodside A (2011) Customer experiences with brands: literature review and research directions. Mark Rev 11:205–225

Rahman M (2014) Differentiated brand experience in brand parity through branded branding strategy. J Strateg Mark 22:603–615

Ramaseshan B, Stein A (2014) Connecting the dots between brand experience and brand loyalty: the mediating role of brand personality and brand relationships. J Brand Manag 21:664–683

Rigby D (2011) The future of shopping. Harv Bus Rev 89:65–76

Robertson TS, Gatignon H, Cesareo L (2018) Pop-ups, ephemerality, and consumer experience: the centrality of buzz. J Assoc Consum Res 3:425–439

Rose S, Hair N, Clark M (2011) Online customer experience: a review of the business-to-consumer online purchase context. Int J Manag Rev 13:24–39

Rose S, Clark M, Samouel P, Hair N (2012) Online customer experience in e-retailing: an empirical model of antecedents and outcomes. J Retail 88:308–322

Roswinanto W, Strutton D (2014) Investigating the advertising antecedents to and consequences of brand experience. J Promot Manag 20:607–627

Salmon P (1989) Personal stances in learning. In: Weil SW, McGill I (eds) Making sense of experiential learning: diversity in theory and practice. The Open University Press, Milton Keynes, pp 230–241

Schembri S, Sandberg J (2002) Service quality and the consumer’s experience: towards an interpretive approach. Mark Theory 2:189–205

Schivinski B, Christodoulides G, Dabrowski D (2016) Measuring consumers’ engagement with brand-related social-media content. J Advert Res 56:64–80

Schmitt B (1999) Experiential marketing. J Mark Manag 15:53–67

Schmitt B (2000) Creating and managing brand experiences on the internet. Des Manag J 11:53–58

Schmitt B, Joško Brakus J, Zarantonello L (2015) From experiential psychology to consumer experience. J Consum Psychol 25:166–171

Scholz J, Smith AN (2016) Augmented reality: designing immersive experiences that maximize consumer engagement. Bus Horiz 59:149–161

Schouten JW, McAlexander JH, Koenig HF (2007) Transcendent customer experience and brand community. J Acad Mark Sci 35:357–368

Shaw C, Ivens J (2002) Building great customer experiences, vol 241. Palgrave, London

Sherry JF, Kozinets RV (2007) Comedy of the commons: nomadic spirituality and the Burning Man festival. In: Belk RW, Sherry JF (eds) Consumer culture theory. JAI Press, Oxford, pp 119–147

Shimp TA, Andrews JC (2013) Advertising, promotion, and other aspects of integrated marketing communications, 9th edn. Cengage Learning, Mason

Singh S, Sonnenburg S (2012) Brand performances in social media. J Interact Mark 26:189–197

Skadberg YX, Kimmel JR (2004) Visitors’ flow experience while browsing a web site: its measurement, contributing factors and consequences. Comput Hum Behav 20:403–422

Smith S, Wheeler J (2002) Managing the customer experience: turning customers into advocates. Financial Times/Prentice Hall, Upper Saddle River

Syrdal HA, Briggs E (2018) Engagement with social media content: a qualitative exploration. J Mark theory Pract 26:4–22

Tafesse W (2016a) Conceptualization of brand experience in an event marketing context. J Promot Manag 22:34–48

Tafesse W (2016b) An experiential model of consumer engagement in social media. J Prod Brand Manag 25:424–434

Takatalo J, Nyman G, Laaksonen L (2008) Components of human experience in virtual environments. Comput Hum Behav 24:1–15

Tax SS, Brown SW, Chandrashekaran M (1998) Customer evaluations of service complaint experiences: implications for relationship marketing. J Mark 62:60–76

Thompson CJ, Locander WB, Pollio HR (1989) Putting consumer experience back into consumer research: the philosophy and method of existential-phenomenology. J Consum Res 16:133–146

Thorbjørnsen H, Supphellen M, Nysveen H, Egil P (2002) Building brand relationships online: a comparison of two interactive applications. J Interact Mark 16:17–34

Trevinal AM, Stenger T (2014) Toward a conceptualization of the online shopping experience. J Retail Consum Serv 21:314–326

Triantafillidou A, Siomkos G (2018) The impact of facebook experience on consumers’ behavioral brand engagement. J Res Interact Mark 12:164–192

Turner P (2017) A psychology of user experience: involvement, Affect and Aesthetics. Springer, Cham

Underwood LG, Teresi JA (2002) The daily spiritual experience scale: development, theoretical description, reliability, exploratory factor analysis, and preliminary construct validity using health-related data. Ann Behav Med 24:22–33

Van Noort G, Voorveld HA, Van Reijmersdal EA (2012) Interactivity in brand web sites: cognitive, affective, and behavioral responses explained by consumers’ online flow experience. J Interact Mark 26:223–234

Vargo SL, Lusch RF (2008) Service-dominant logic: continuing the evolution. J Acad Mark Sci 36:1–10

Verhoef PC, Lemon KN, Parasuraman A, Roggeveen A, Tsiros M, Schlesinger LA (2009) Customer experience creation: determinants, dynamics and management strategies. J Retail 85:31–41

Walls A, Okumus F, Wang Y, Kwun DJ-W (2011) Understanding the consumer experience: an exploratory study of luxury hotels. J Hosp Mark Manag 20:166–197

Wan Y, Nakayama M, Sutcliffe N (2012) The impact of age and shopping experiences on the classification of search, experience, and credence goods in online shopping. Inf Syst E bus Manag 10:135–148

Whitener EM (2001) Do “high commitment” human resource practices affect employee commitment? A cross-level analysis using hierarchical linear modeling. J Manag 27:515–535

Won Jeong S, Fiore AM, Niehm LS, Lorenz FO (2009) The role of experiential value in online shopping: the impacts of product presentation on consumer responses towards an apparel web site. Internet Res 19:105–124

Wooten DB, Reed A II (1998) Informational influence and the ambiguity of product experience: order effects on the weighting of evidence. J Consum Psychol 7:79–99

Yoon D, Youn S (2016) Brand experience on the website: its mediating role between perceived interactivity and relationship quality. J Interact Advert 16:1–15

Zarantonello L, Schmitt BH (2010) Using the brand experience scale to profile consumers and predict consumer behaviour. J Brand Manag 17:532–540

Zarantonello L, Schmitt BH (2013) The impact of event marketing on brand equity: the mediating roles of brand experience and brand attitude. Int J Advert 32:255–280

Zenetti G, Klapper D (2016) Advertising effects under consumer heterogeneity–the moderating role of brand experience, advertising recall and attitude. J Retail 92:352–372

Zhang H, Lu Y, Gupta S, Zhao L (2014) What motivates customers to participate in social commerce? The impact of technological environments and virtual customer experiences. Inf Manag 51:1017–1030

Zhang H, Lu Y, Wang B, Wu S (2015) The impacts of technological environments and co-creation experiences on customer participation. Inf Manag 52:468–482

Zolfagharian M, Jordan AT (2007) Multiracial identity and art consumption. In: Belk RW, Sherry JF Jr (eds) Consumer culture theory. JAI Press, Oxford

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank editor-in-chief (Prof. Dr Joern Block), and the anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments on this paper.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Marketing, Faculty of Business and Accountancy, Graduate School of Business, University of Malaya, Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia

Muhammad Waqas & Zalfa Laili Binti Hamzah

Department of Operation and Management Information System, Faculty of Business and Accountancy, Graduate School of Business, University of Malaya, Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia

Noor Akma Mohd Salleh

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Zalfa Laili Binti Hamzah .

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

See Table  8 .

See Table  9 .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Waqas, M., Hamzah, Z.L.B. & Salleh, N.A.M. Customer experience: a systematic literature review and consumer culture theory-based conceptualisation. Manag Rev Q 71 , 135–176 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11301-020-00182-w

Download citation

Received : 08 May 2019

Accepted : 10 February 2020

Published : 15 February 2020

Issue Date : February 2021

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s11301-020-00182-w

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Customer experience
  • Literature review
  • Consumer culture theory
  • Online experience

JEL Classification

  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

To read this content please select one of the options below:

Please note you do not have access to teaching notes, artificial intelligence applications to customer feedback research: a review.

Artificial Intelligence in Marketing

ISBN : 978-1-80262-876-0 , eISBN : 978-1-80262-875-3

Publication date: 13 March 2023

In this paper, we aim to provide a comprehensive overview of customer feedback literature, highlighting the burgeoning role of artificial intelligence (AI). Customer feedback has long been a valuable source of customer insights for businesses and market researchers. While previously survey focused, customer feedback in the digital age has evolved to be rich, interactive, multimodal, and virtually real time. Such explosion in feedback content has also been accompanied by a rapid development of AI and machine learning technologies that enable firms to understand and take advantage of these high-velocity data sources. Yet, some of the challenges with traditional surveys remain, such as self-selection concerns of who chooses to participate and what attributes they give feedback on. In addition, these new feedback channels face other unique challenges like review manipulation and herding effects due to their public and democratic nature. Thus, while the AI toolkit has revolutionized the area of customer feedback, extracting meaningful insights requires complementing it with the appropriate social science toolkit. We begin by touching upon conventional customer feedback research and chart its evolution through the years as the nature of available data and analysis tools develop. We conclude by providing recommendations for future questions that remain to be explored in this field.

  • Customer feedback
  • Artificial intelligence
  • Reputation systems
  • Unstructured data
  • Machine learning
  • Natural language processing
  • Causal inference

Lee, P.S. , Chakraborty, I. and Banerjee, S. (2023), "Artificial Intelligence Applications to Customer Feedback Research: A Review", Sudhir, K. and Toubia, O. (Ed.) Artificial Intelligence in Marketing ( Review of Marketing Research, Vol. 20 ), Emerald Publishing Limited, Leeds, pp. 169-190. https://doi.org/10.1108/S1548-643520230000020010

Emerald Publishing Limited

Copyright © 2023 Peter S. Lee, Ishita Chakraborty and Shrabastee Banerjee. Published under exclusive licence by Emerald Publishing Limited

We’re listening — tell us what you think

Something didn’t work….

Report bugs here

All feedback is valuable

Please share your general feedback

Join us on our journey

Platform update page.

Visit emeraldpublishing.com/platformupdate to discover the latest news and updates

Questions & More Information

Answers to the most commonly asked questions here

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

Exploring the Impact of Customer Feedback on the Well-Being of Service Entities: A TSR Perspective

Profile image of Jamie Burton

2014, Journal of Service Management

Purpose – Adopting the transformative service research (TSR) perspective, the purpose of this paper is to investigate the impact of positive customer feedback on the well-being of front-line employees, companies, and society. Using a multidisciplinary approach, the overlooked area of “positive feedback” is explored resulting in the development of the “Positive Feedback Model” (PFM). The study also compares managers’ and employees’ perceptions of positive customer feedback. Design/methodology/approach – Two exploratory qualitative studies were conducted: Study 1 consisted of 22 semi-structured interviews with managers working in the service industry and Study 2 consisted of seven focus groups with front-line service employees. The extensive literature review and the results of these two studies contributed to the development of the PFM. Findings – Positive customer feedback is an overlooked area of service research which offers potential for improving the well-being of the service entities. Front-line employees are the main recipients and topics of positive customer feedback. The developed PFM describes various forms, channels, and times of administration of positive customer feedback and its multitude of impacts on the well-being of service entities. Research limitations/implications – This study contributes to the literature on TSR and customer feedback management. The developed model presents possible positive feedback categories, their various outcomes and the outcomes for the concerned parties involved. By developing PFM and encouraging a multidisciplinary approach combined with advanced research methodologies, the researchers propose an agenda for further research insights within the TSR and customer feedback areas. The comparison of the managers’ and employees’ perceptions of positive customer feedback presents novel managerial implications and directions for future research. Originality/value – This study is the first to explore customer feedback from a TSR perspective. It examines the overlooked area of positive customer feedback. The well-being of service entities is prioritized as services have been extensively criticized for ignoring human well-being.

Related Papers

Journal of Service Management

jamie burton

customer feedback research paper

Journal of Services Marketing

Jamie Burton

Purpose – Front-line employee (FLE) well-being is an under-researched field. Contrasting the prevailing view that Positive Customer Feedback (PCF) can only have ‘positive’ impacts, this study aims to answer the counterintuitive question: Could the apparently positive construct ‘Positive Customer Feedback’ have a negative impact on the well-being of front-line employees? Consequently, working within the Transformative Service Research (TSR) framework, we investigate whether PCF can negatively affect the eudaimonic and hedonic well-being dimensions of FLEs, thus decreasing their overall psychological well-being level. Design/methodology/approach – A multidisciplinary literature review was conducted, particularly in the social psychology, human resources and organizational behavior fields, to examine the potential negative impacts of PCF. Subsequently, an exploratory qualitative study consisting of seven focus groups with 45 FLEs and 22 in-depth interviews with managers working across ...

Journal of Business Communication

Martha Andrews

Employee Assistance Quarterly

Mark S . Adams

Trude Furunes

International Journal of Contemporary Hospitality Management

Edwin Arrigui Torres

International e-Conference , Dr. Annie Babah Alargi

Purpose: The purpose of this paper was to investigate how data from customer feedback is utilized by the Telco and translated into strategies and competency based actions to address customer needs and service concerns. Problem statement: Whereas customers are displeased with cost of services, limited network service coverage, long queues and poor connectivity, this Telco have focused on basic elements of service including greeting, smiling, making the customer feel comfortable at their service centres and enticing them with discounts and promotions. Methodology: A mixed method of qualitative and quantitative research was used. Using purposive sampling, two homogeneous groups were selected; the first was semi-structured interviews with five managers. Secondly, closed ended questionnaires were dispensed to a quota sample of 20 affluent customers. Findings: This Telco giant mainly collects customer feedback through call centres, social media, direct customer engagement, close-loop feedback system. However the feedback is not adequately utilized because it does not favour the Telco relatively to profit optimization and in some cases involve new system infrastructure. Research limitations: The study focused on one giant Telco within the industry in Ghana believed to have best practices that can be generalised to other Telcos in market. Selection of only affluent customers represents a deeper understanding of customer satisfaction issues. Practical implications: whereas this Telco believes that it is delivering the best of service to its customers by smiling at them, greeting, making them feel welcome and rewarding them with promotional gifts and discounts, the customers on the other hand, believes that the service the Telcos are offering are “must be” and that, these are basics of service, hence what they expect including improved network coverage, decrease in high cost of voice calls, seamless connectivity and zero interruptions in service transactions are not what they are actually experiencing to enable their satisfaction. Originality: While this paper is the first to consider customer feedback utilization of a conglomerate Telco in Ghana, it also measured the service competency based actions that is implemented to provide superior service experiences to its customers. Keywords: customer service experience, service value, service management, customer expectations, customer desires, customer delight.

Nursing times

Derek Milne

Tourism Management

Tijana Radojević

RELATED PAPERS

MOON카지노〃〃GCN777。COM〃〃강원랜드카지노

Harga Paving Block Termurah

Martin D Liu

Alexandre Patriota

The Numismatic chronicle

Matthew Ponting

Buletin Peternakan

Tri Retno Dyah Larasati

Muhammad Waseem Ur Rehman

Pablo Martel

New J. Chem.

Zia Ur rehman

Geoheritage

Iván Martín Rojas

Research Article

Dr. Hafiz M U H A M M A D H A S S A N Mahmood

Molecular Biology of …

Philippe Benaroch

Revista Mulemba

Tania Macedo

Interface - Comunicação, Saúde, Educação

Magda Duarte Dos Anjos Scherer

Jurnal Teknik Pertanian Lampung (Journal of Agricultural Engineering)

Heri Purwanto

Muhammad 'Aqil 'Azib Mohd Zailani

Fruzsina Cseh

MIST INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF SCIENCE AND TECHNOLOGY

Sakil Ahmed

Chemosphere

neil riordan

Revista del Hospital …

Marta Bertin

The Journal of Animal and …

Benedict Offem

Dinamika: Jurnal Ilmiah Pendidikan Dasar

desi setiyadi

Journal of Human, Earth, and Future

Muhammad Waqas Hussain

Transfusion

Vaijayanti Kale

See More Documents Like This

RELATED TOPICS

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024
  • Browse All Articles
  • Newsletter Sign-Up

CustomerSatisfaction →

No results found in working knowledge.

  • Were any results found in one of the other content buckets on the left?
  • Try removing some search filters.
  • Use different search filters.

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List

Logo of plosone

The future of feedback: Motivating performance improvement through future-focused feedback

Jackie Gnepp

1 Humanly Possible, Inc., Oak Park, Illinois, United States of America

Joshua Klayman

2 Booth School of Business, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois, United States of America

Ian O. Williamson

3 Wellington School of Business and Government, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand

Sema Barlas

4 Masters of Science in Analytics, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois, United States of America

Associated Data

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files ( S1 Dataset ).

Managerial feedback discussions often fail to produce the desired performance improvements. Three studies shed light on why performance feedback fails and how it can be made more effective. In Study 1, managers described recent performance feedback experiences in their work settings. In Studies 2 and 3, pairs of managers role-played a performance review meeting. In all studies, recipients of mixed and negative feedback doubted the accuracy of the feedback and the providers’ qualifications to give it. Disagreement regarding past performance was greater following the feedback discussion than before, due to feedback recipients’ increased self-protective and self-enhancing attributions. Managers were motivated to improve to the extent they perceived the feedback conversation to be focused on future actions rather than on past performance. Our findings have implications for the theory and practice of performance management.

Introduction

Once again, Taylor Devani is hoping to be promoted to Regional Manager. Chris Sinopoli, Taylor’s new boss, has arranged a meeting to provide performance feedback, especially regarding ways Taylor must change to succeed in a Regional Manager position. Like Taylor’s previous boss, Chris is delighted with Taylor’s award-winning sales performance. But Taylor was admonished in last year’s performance appraisal about cavalier treatment of customers and intolerant behavior toward employees. Taylor was very resistant to that message then and there have been no noticeable improvements since. What can Chris say to get through to Taylor?

This vignette highlights three points that will be familiar to theorists, researchers, and practitioners of performance feedback. First, the vignette reflects that performance feedback often includes a mix of both positive and negative feedback. Second, it reflects the common experience that the recipients do not always accept the feedback they get, let alone act on it. Third, it raises the question of what a feedback provider should say (and perhaps not say) in order to enable and motivate the feedback recipient to improve.

The present research focuses on feedback conversations in the context of work and career, but it has implications far beyond those contexts. Giving feedback about performance is one of the key elements of mentorship, coaching, supervision, and parenting. It contributes to conflict resolution in intimate relationships [ 1 ] and it is considered one of the most powerful activities in education [ 2 ]. In all these instances, the primary goal is to motivate and direct positive behavior change. Thus, a better understanding of where performance feedback conversations go wrong and how they can be made more effective is an important contribution to the psychology of work and to organizational psychology, but also to a broad range of psychological literatures, including education, consulting, counseling, and interpersonal communications.

Across three studies, we provide the first evidence that performance feedback discussions can have counterproductive effects by increasing the recipient’s self-serving attributions for past performance, thereby decreasing agreement between the providers and recipients of feedback. These unintended effects are associated with lower feedback acceptance and with lower motivation to change. Our studies also provide the first empirical evidence that feedback discussions promote intentions to act on the feedback to the extent they are perceived as focusing on future performance, rather than past performance. These findings suggest a new line of investigation for a topic with a long and venerable history.

Performance feedback in the workplace

Performance feedback can be distinguished from other types of managerial feedback (e.g., “production is up 12% from last quarter”) by its focus on the recipients’ conduct and accomplishments–doing the right things the right way with the right results. It is nearly universal in the modern workplace. Even the recent trend toward doing away with annual performance reviews has come with a directive for managers to have more frequent, if less formal, performance feedback conversations [ 3 ].

Psychologists have known for decades that the effects of performance feedback on performance are highly variable and not always beneficial: A meta-analysis by Kluger and DeNisi found that the modal impact on performance is none [ 4 ]. Such findings fostered a focus on employee reactions to performance appraisals and the idea that employees would be motivated to change behavior only if they accepted the feedback and believed there was a need to improve [ 5 – 7 ]. Unfortunately, unfavorable feedback is not easily accepted. People have been shown to cope with negative feedback by disputing it, lowering their goals, reducing commitment, misremembering or reinterpreting the feedback to be more positive, and engaging in self-esteem repair, none of which are likely to motivate efforts to do a better job next time [ 8 – 16 ].

We are not recommending that feedback providers avoid negative feedback in favor of positive. Glossing over discrepancies between actual performance and desired standards of performance is not a satisfactory solution: Both goal-setting theory and ample evidence support the idea that people need summary feedback comparing progress to goals in order to adjust their efforts and strategies to reach those standards or goals [ 17 , 18 ]. The solution we propose is feedback that focuses less on diagnosing past performance and more on designing future performance.

Diagnosing the past

Managers talk to employees about both the nature and the determinants of their performance, often with the goal of improving that performance. Indeed, feedback theorists have long argued that managers must diagnose the causes of past performance problems in order to generate insight into what skills people need to improve and how they should change [ 19 ]. Understanding root causes is believed to help everyone decide future action.

Yet causality is ambiguous in performance situations. Both feedback providers and feedback recipients make causal attributions for performance that are biased, albeit in different ways. Whereas the correspondence bias leads the feedback provider to over-attribute success and failure alike to qualities of the employee [ 20 – 22 ], this bias is modified by a self-serving bias for the feedback recipient. Specifically, feedback recipients are more inclined to attribute successes to their positive dispositional qualities, and failures to external forces such as bad luck and situational constraints [ 23 – 26 ]. These self-enhancing and self-protective attributions benefit both affect and feelings of self-worth [ 27 , 28 ].

Organizational scholars have theorized since the 1970’s that such attribution differences between leaders and subordinates are a likely source of conflict and miscommunication in performance reviews [ 12 , 29 – 31 ]. Despite this solid basis in social psychological theory, little evidence exists regarding the prevalence and significance of attribution misalignment in the context of everyday workplace feedback. In the workplace, where people tend to trust their colleagues, have generally positive supervisor-supervisee relations they wish to maintain, and where feedback often takes place within a longer history of interaction, there may be more agreement about the causes of past events than seen in experimental settings. In Study 1, we explored whether attribution disagreement is indeed prevalent in the workplace by surveying hundreds of managers working in hundreds of different settings in which they gave or received positive or negative feedback. (In this paper, “disagreement” refers to a difference of opinion and is not meant to imply an argument between parties.) If workplace results mirror experimental findings and the organizational theorizing reviewed above, then our survey should reveal that when managers receive negative feedback, they make more externally focused attributions and they view that feedback as lacking credibility.

Can feedback discussions lead the two parties to a consensual understanding of the recipient’s past performance, so that its quality can be sustained or improved? One would be hard pressed these days to find a feedback theorist who did not advocate two-way communication in delivering feedback. Shouldn’t the two parties expect to converge on the “truth” of the matter through a sharing of perspectives? Gioia and Sims asked managers to make attributions for subordinates’ performance both before and after giving feedback [ 32 ]. Following the feedback conversation, managers gave more credit for success and less blame for failure. However, Gioia and Simms did not assess whether the recipients of feedback were influenced to think differently about their performance and that, after all, is the point of giving feedback.

Should one expect the recipients of workplace feedback to meet the providers halfway, taking less credit for success and/or more responsibility for failure following the feedback discussion? There are reasons to suspect not. The self-serving tendency in attributions is magnified under conditions of self-threat, that is, when information is conveyed that questions, contradicts, or challenges a person’s favorable view of the self [ 33 ]. People mentally argue against threatening feedback, rejecting what they find refutable [ 11 , 34 ]. In Studies 2 and 3, we explored the effects of live feedback discussions on attributions, feedback acceptance, and motivation to improve. We anticipated that feedback recipients would find their self-serving tendencies magnified by hearing feedback that challenged their favorable self-views. We hypothesized that the very act of discussing performance would create or exacerbate differences of opinion about what caused past performance, rather than reduce them. We expected this divergence in attributions to result in recipients rejecting the feedback and questioning the legitimacy of the source, conditions that render feedback ineffective for motivating improvement [ 7 , 14 , 35 ].

Focusing on the future

Given the psychological obstacles to people’s acceptance of negative feedback, how can managers lead their subordinates to want to change their behavior and improve their performance? This question lies at the heart of the challenge posed by feedback discussions intended both to inform people and motivate them, sometimes referred to as “developmental” feedback. Despite its intended focus on learning and improvement [ 36 , 37 ], developmental feedback may nonetheless explicitly include a diagnostic focus on the past [ 38 ], such as “why the subjects thought that they had done so poorly, what aspects of the task they had difficulty with, and what they thought their strong points were” (p. 32). In contrast, we propose that the solution lies in focusing on the future: We suggest that ideas generated by a focus on future possibilities are more effective at motivating change than are ideas generated by diagnosing why things went well or poorly in the past. This hypothesis is based on recent theory and findings regarding prospective thinking and planning.

Much prospection (mentally simulating the future) is pragmatic in that it involves thinking about practical actions one can take and behavioral changes one can make to bring about desirable future outcomes [ 39 ]. In the context of mixed or negative performance feedback, such desirable outcomes might include improved performance, better results, and greater rewards. Research comparing forward to backward thinking suggests that people find it easier to come up with practical solutions to problems in the future than to imagine practical ways problems could have been avoided in the past: People are biased toward seeing past events as inevitable, finding it difficult to imagine how things might have turned out differently [ 40 – 42 ]. When thinking about their past failures, people tend to focus on how things beyond their control could have been better (e.g., they might have had fewer competing responsibilities and more resources). In contrast, when thinking about how their performance could be more successful in the future, people focus on features under their control, generating more goal-directed thoughts [ 43 ]. Thinking through the steps needed to achieve desired goals makes change in the future feel more feasible [ 44 ]. And when success seems feasible, contrasting the past with the future leads people to take more responsibility, initiate actions, engage in effortful striving, and achieve more of their goals, as compared to focusing on past difficulties [ 45 ]. For all these reasons, we hypothesize that more prospective, forward looking feedback conversations will motivate intentions toward positive change.

Overview of studies

We report three studies. The first explored the prevalence and consequences of differing attributional perspectives in the workplace. Managers described actual, recently experienced incidents of work-related feedback and the degree to which they accepted that feedback as legitimate. The second study was designed to examine and question the pervasive view that a two-way feedback discussion leads the parties to a shared explanation of past performance and a shared desire for behavior change. We hypothesized instead that the attributions of feedback providers and recipients diverge as a consequence of reviewing past performance. In that study, businesspeople role-played a performance review meeting based on objective data in a personnel file. The third study is a modified replication of the second, with an added emphasis on the developmental purpose of the feedback. Finally, we used data from Studies 2 and 3 to model the connections among provider-recipient attribution differences, future focus, feedback acceptance, and intentions to change. Our overarching theory posits that in the workplace (and in other domains of life), feedback conversations are most beneficial when they avoid the diagnosis of the past and instead focus directly on implications for future action.

We conducted an international survey of managers who described recent work-based incidents in which they either provided or received feedback, positive or negative. We explored how the judgmental biases documented in attribution research are manifested in everyday feedback conversations and how those biases relate to acceptance of feedback. Given well-established phenomena of attribution (correspondence bias, actor-observer differences, self-serving bias), we expected managers to favor internal attributions for the events that prompted the feedback, except for incidents in which they received negative feedback. We hypothesized that managers who received negative feedback would, furthermore, judge the feedback as less accurate and the feedback providers as less qualified, when compared to managers who received positive feedback or who provided feedback of either valence.

Participants

Respondents to this survey were 419 middle and upper managers enrolled in Executive MBA classes in Chicago, Barcelona, and Singapore. They represented a mix of American, European, and Asian businesspeople. Females comprised 18% of participants. For procedural reasons (see Results), the responses of 37 participants were excluded from analysis, leaving a sample of 382. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Chicago, which waived the requirement for written consent as was its customary policy for studies judged to be of minimal risk, involving only individual, anonymized survey responses.

Managers completed the survey online, using the Cogix ViewsFlash survey platform. When they accessed the survey, they were randomly assigned to one of four conditions. Each participant was instructed to think of one recent work-related incident in which they gave another person positive feedback (provider-positive condition), gave another person negative feedback (provider-negative condition), received positive feedback from another person (recipient-positive condition), or received negative feedback from another person (recipient-negative condition). They were asked to describe briefly the incident and the feedback.

The managers were then asked to complete the statement, “The feedback was __% accurate,” and to rate the qualification of the feedback provider on a scale from 0 = unqualified to 10 = completely qualified. Providers were asked, “How qualified were you to give the feedback?” whereas recipients were asked, “The person who gave you the feedback—how qualified was he or she to give the feedback?”

Lastly, the managers were instructed to make causal attributions for the incident. They were told, “Looking back now at the incident, please assign a percentage to each of the following causes, such that they sum to 100%.” Two of the causes corresponded to Weiner’s internal attribution categories (ability and effort) [ 28 ]. The other two causes corresponded to Weiner’s external attribution categories (task and luck). The wording of the response choices varied with condition. For example, in the provider-positive condition, the response choices were __% due to abilities he or she possessed, __% due to the amount of effort he or she put in, __% due to the nature of what he or she had to do, __% due to good luck, whereas for the recipient-negative condition, the attribution choices were __% due to abilities you lacked, __% due to the amount of effort you put in, __% due to the nature of what you had to do, __% due to bad luck. (Full text is provided in S1 Text .)

A review of the incidents and feedback the participants described revealed that 25 managers had violated instructions by writing about incidents that were not work-related (e.g., interactions with family members) and 12 had written about incidents inconsistent with their assigned condition (e.g., describing feedback received when assigned to a feedback provider condition). The data from these 37 managers were excluded from further analysis, leaving samples of 96, 92, 91, and 103 in the provider-positive, provider-negative, recipient-positive, and recipient-negative conditions, respectively. We tested the data using ANOVAs with role (providing vs. receiving feedback) and valence (positive vs. negative feedback) as between-subjects variables.

There were three dependent variables: managers’ ratings of feedback accuracy, of provider qualifications, and of internal vs. external causal attributions (ability + effort vs. task + luck). Analyses of the attribution variable used the arcsine transformation commonly recommended for proportions [ 46 ]. For all three dependent measures, there were significant main effects of role and valence and a significant interaction between them (see Table 1 and Fig 1 ).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0234444.g001.jpg

Results for each dependent variable are shown by role (provider vs. recipient of feedback) and valence (positive vs. negative feedback). Error bars show standard errors.

All F (1, 378), all p < .001; effect size measures are partial η 2 . Correlations among dependent measures are shown in S1 Table .

Providers of feedback reported that the incidents in question were largely caused by the abilities and efforts of the feedback recipients. They reported that their feedback was accurate and that they were well qualified to give it. These findings held for both positive and negative feedback. Recipients of feedback made similar judgments when the feedback was positive: They took personal credit for incidents that turned out well and accepted the positive feedback as true. However, when the feedback was negative, recipients judged the failures as due principally to causes beyond their control, such as task demands and bad luck. They did not accept the negative feedback received, judging it as less accurate ( t (192) = 7.50, p < .001) and judging the feedback provider less qualified to give it t (192) = 5.25, p < .001). One manager who defended the reasonableness of these findings during a group debrief put it this way: “We are the best there is. If we get negative feedback for something bad that happened, it probably wasn’t our fault!”

Study 1 confirms that attributional disagreement is prevalent in the workplace and associated with the rejection of negative feedback. Across a large sample of real, recent, work-related incidents, providers and recipients of feedback formed very different impressions of both the feedback and the incidents that prompted it. Despite the general tendency of people to attribute the causes of performance to internal factors such as ability and effort, managers who received negative feedback placed most of the blame outside themselves. Our survey further confirmed that, across a wide variety of workplace settings, managers who received negative feedback viewed it as lacking credibility, rating the feedback as less accurate and the source as less qualified to provide feedback.

These results are consistent with attribution theory and the fact that feedback providers and recipients have access to different information: Whereas providers have an external perspective on the recipients’ observable behavior, feedback recipients have unique access to their own thoughts, feelings, and intentions, all of which drove their performance and behavior [ 24 , 47 ]. For the most part, feedback recipients intend to perform well. When their efforts pay off, they perceive they had personal control over the positive outcome; when their efforts fail, they naturally look for causes outside themselves [ 48 , 49 ]. For their part, feedback providers are prone to paying insufficient attention to situational constraints, even when motivated to give honest, accurate, unbiased, and objective feedback [ 20 ].

In this survey study, every incident was unique: Providers and recipients were not reporting on the same incidents. Thus, the survey method permits an additional mechanism of self-protection, namely, biased selection of congenial information [ 50 ]. When faced with a request to recall a recent incident that resulted in receipt of negative feedback, the managers may have tended to retrieve incidents for which they were not to blame and that did not reflect poorly on their abilities. Such biased recall often occurs outside of conscious awareness [ 51 , 52 ]. For the recipients of feedback, internal attributions for the target incident have direct implications for self-esteem. Thus, they may have tended to recall incidents aligned with their wish to maintain a positive self-view, namely, successes due to ability and effort, and failures due to task demands and bad luck. It is possible, of course, that providers engaged in selective recall as well: They may have enhanced their sense of competence and fairness by retrieving incidents in which they were highly qualified and provided accurate feedback. Biased selection of incidents is not possible in the next two studies which provided all participants with identical workplace-performance information.

In Study 2 we investigated how and how much the feedback conversation itself alters the two parties’ judgments of the performance under discussion. This study tests our hypotheses that feedback discussions do not lead to greater agreement about attributions and may well lead to increased disagreement, that attributional misalignment is associated with rejection of feedback, and that future focus is associated with greater feedback effectiveness, as measured by acceptance of feedback and intention to change. The study used a dyadic role-play simulation of a performance review meeting in which a supervisor (newly hired Regional Manager Chris Sinopoli) gives performance feedback to a subordinate (District Manager Taylor Devani, being considered for promotion). The simulation was adapted from a performance feedback exercise that is widely used in management training. Instructors and researchers who use similar role-play exercises report that participants find them realistic and engaging, and respond as they would to the real thing [ 32 , 53 ].

The decision to use a role-play method involves trade-offs, especially when compared to studying in vivo workplace performance reviews. We chose this method in order to gain greater experimental control and a cleaner test of our hypotheses. In our study, all participants were given identical information, in the form of a personnel file, ensuring that both the providers and recipients of feedback based their judgements on the same information. This control would not be possible inside an actual company, where the two parties might easily be influenced by differential access to organizational knowledge and different exposure to the events under discussion. Additionally, participants in our study completed questionnaires that assessed their perceptions of the feedback-recipient’s performance, the discussion of that performance, and the effects of the feedback discussion. Because this study was a simulation, participants were able to respond honestly to these questionnaires. Participants in an actual workplace performance review might need to balance honesty with concerns for appearances or repercussions; for example, feedback recipients might be hesitant to admit having little intention to change in response to feedback. On the other hand, there are a variety of conditions and motivations that exist in the workplace that cannot be easily simulated in a role-play, such as the pre-existing relationship between the feedback provider and recipient, and the potential long-term consequences of any performance review. Further work will be required to determine how findings from this study apply in workplace settings.

This study comprised two groups that received the same scenarios, but differed with regard to the timing and content of the questionnaires. Recall that the primary goal of Study 2 was to explore how the feedback discussion affects participants’ judgments. For this, we analyzed data from the pre-post group. Participants in this group completed questionnaires both before and after the discussion. Their post-discussion questionnaire included questions evaluating the conduct and consequences of the feedback discussion, including ratings of future focus and intention to change. A second group of participants (the post-only group) completed only a questionnaire after the feedback discussion that did not include future-focus or intention-to-change items. This group allowed us to test whether answering the same questions twice (pre and post the feedback discussion) affected the results.

Participants were 380 executives and MBA students enrolled in advanced Human Resources classes in Australia. They represented an international mix of businesspeople: 59% identified their “main cultural identity” as Australian, 20% as a European nationality or ethnicity, 24% Asian, and 12% other; 5% did not indicate any. (Totals sum to more than 100% because participants were able to choose two identities if they wished.) They averaged 35 years of age, ranging from 23 to 66. Females comprised 35% of the sample. Participants worked in pairs. Five pairs were excluded from analysis because one member of the dyad did not complete the required questionnaires, leaving a sample of 117 dyads in the pre-post group and 68 in the post-only group. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Melbourne. Participants’ written consent was obtained.

Each participant received a packet of materials consisting of (a) background on a fictional telecommunications company called the DeltaCom Corporation, (b) a description of both their role and their partner’s role, (c) task instructions for completing the questionnaires and the role-play itself, (d) a copy of the personnel file for the subordinate, and (e) the questionnaire(s). The names of the role-play characters were pre-tested to be gender neutral. (The full text of the materials is provided in S2 – S7 Texts .)

Personnel file . The personnel file documented a mixed record including both exemplary and problematic aspects of the District Manger’s performance. On the positive side was superior, award-winning sales performance and consistently above-average increases in new customers. On the negative side were consistently below-average ratings of customer satisfaction and a falling percentage of customers retained, along with high turnover of direct reports, some of whom complained of the District Manager’s “moody, tyrannical, and obsessive” behavior. Notes from the prior year’s performance discussion indicated that the District Manager did not fully accept the developmental feedback received at that time, instead defending a focus on sales success and the bottom line.

Questionnaires . Participants in the pre-post group completed a pre-discussion questionnaire immediately following their review of the District Manager’s personnel file. They rated the quality of the District Manager’s job performance on sales, customer retention, customer satisfaction, and ability to manage and coach employees, using 7-point scales ranging from 1 = Very Low to 7 = Very High. They then rated the importance of these four aspects of the recipient’s job performance on 7-point scales ranging from 1 = Not Important to 7 = Very Important. Lastly, participants gave their “opinion about the causes of Taylor Devani’s successes by assigning a percentage to each of the following four causes, such that the four causes together sum to 100%.” They did the same for “Taylor Devani’s failures.” Two response categories described internal attributions: “% due to Taylor’s abilities and personality” and “% due to the amount of effort and attention Taylor applied.” The other two described external attributions: “% due to Taylor’s job responsibilities, DeltaCom’s expectations, and the resources provided” and “% due to chance and random luck.” (We chose the expression “random luck” to imply uncontrollable environmental factors in contrast to a trait or feature of a lucky or unlucky person [ 54 ].) Participants chose a percentage from 0 to 100 for each cause, using scales in increments of 5 percentage points. In 4.4% of cases, participants’ four attribution ratings summed to a total, T , that did not equal 100. In those cases, all the ratings were adjusted by multiplying by (100 / T ).

Participants in both the pre-post group and the post-only group completed a post-discussion questionnaire following their feedback discussion. This questionnaire asked the participants to rate the favorability of the feedback given, on an 11-point scale from 0 = “Almost all negative” to 10 = “Almost all positive”; the accuracy of the feedback, on a scale from 0% to 100% in increments of 5%; and how qualified the provider was to give the feedback, on an 11-point scale from 0 = “Unqualified” to 10 = “Completely qualified.” It continued by asking all of the pre-discussion questionnaire items, allowing us to assess any rating changes that occurred in the pre-post group as a consequence of the intervening feedback discussion. Next, for those in the pre-post group, the questionnaire presented a series of 7-point Likert-scale items concerning the conduct and consequences of the feedback. These included items evaluating future focus and intention to change. Additionally, the post-discussion questionnaires of both groups contained exploratory questions about the behaviors of the individual role-players; these were not analyzed. On the final page, participants provided demographic information about themselves.

Participants were randomly assigned to dyads and to roles within each dyad. They were sent to private study rooms to complete the procedure. Instructions indicated (a) 15 minutes to review the personnel file, (b) 5 minutes to complete the pre-discussion questionnaire (pre-post group only), (c) 20 minutes to hold the feedback discussion, and (d) 15 minutes to complete the post-discussion questionnaire. Participants were instructed to stay in role during the entire exercise, including completion of the questionnaires. They were told to complete all steps individually without consulting their partner except, of course, for the feedback discussion. The feedback provider was directed by the task instructions to focus on the recipient’s “weaknesses as a manager–those aspects of performance Taylor must change to achieve future success if promoted.” The reason for this additional instruction was to balance the discussion of successes and failures. Prior pilot testing showed that without this instruction there was a tendency for role-players to avoid discussing shortcomings at all, a finding consistent with research showing that people are reluctant to deliver negative feedback and sometimes distort it to make it more positive [ 35 , 55 – 57 ]. When they finished, the participants handed in all the materials and took part in a group debrief of the performance review simulation.

We used analyses of variance to study differences in how the participants interpreted the past performance of the feedback recipient. The dependent variables were participant judgments of (a) internal vs. external attributions for the feedback recipient’s performance, (b) the quality of various aspects of job performance, and (c) the importance of those aspects. One set of ANOVAs used post-feedback questionnaire data from both the pre-post and post-only groups to check whether completing a pre-discussion questionnaire affected post-discussion results. The independent variables were role (provider or recipient of feedback), outcomes (successes or failures of the feedback recipient), and group (pre-post or post-only). A second set of ANOVAs used data from the pre-discussion and post-discussion questionnaires of the pre-post group to test our hypothesis that feedback discussions tend to drive providers’ and recipients’ interpretations of performance further apart rather than closer together. The independent variables in these analyses were role , outcomes , and timing (before or after feedback conversation). In all the ANOVAs, the dyad was treated as a unit (i.e., as though a single participant) because the responses of the two members of a dyad can hardly be considered independent of one another. Accordingly, role, outcomes, group, and timing were all within-dyad variables.

A third set of analyses provided tests of our hypotheses that provider-recipient disagreement about attributions interferes with feedback effectiveness, and that a focus on future behavior, rather than past behavior, improves feedback effectiveness. We conducted regression analyses using data from the pre-post group, whose questionnaires included the set of Likert-scale items concerning the conduct and consequences of the feedback discussion. The dependent variables for these regressions were two measures of feedback effectiveness derived from recipient responses: the recipients’ acceptance of the feedback as legitimate and the recipients’ expressed intention to change. The predictors represented five characteristics measured from the post-feedback questionnaire: provider-recipient disagreement about attributions, about performance quality, and about performance importance; how favorable the recipient found the feedback to be; and the extent to which the recipient judged the conversation to be future focused.

Role differences in the interpretation of past performance before and after feedback discussion

Given the results of Study 1 and established phenomena in social psychology, we expected feedback recipients to make internal attributions for their successes and external for their failures more than feedback providers do, to hold more favorable views of their job performance quality than providers do, and to see their successes as more important and/or their failures as less important than providers do. Analyses of the post-discussion ratings in the pre-post and post-only groups ( S1 Analyses ) confirm those expectations for attributions and for performance quality, but not for performance importance. There were no differences between the pre-post and post-only groups on any of those measures, with all partial η 2 < .02. Beyond that, we hypothesized that feedback conversations do not reduce provider-recipient differences in interpretation, and may well make them larger. Accordingly, we report here the analyses that include the timing variable, using data from the pre-post group ( Table 2 ).

F (1, 116) for internal attributions, F (1, 114) for performance quality and importance. Underlined values are effects with p < .05 and partial η 2 > .05.

Internal vs . external attributions . Participants in both roles provided attribution ratings before and after the discussion, separately “about the causes of Taylor Devani’s successes” and “about the causes of Taylor Devani’s failures.” There were three significant effects, all of which were interactions. Those were Role x Outcomes, Outcomes x Timing, and Role x Outcomes x Timing. As shown in Fig 2 , the three-way interaction reflects the following pattern: The parties began with only minor (and not statistically significant) differences in attributional perspective. Following the feedback discussion however, those differences were much greater. There were no significant effects involving timing for feedback providers: Their attributions changed only slightly from pre- to post-discussion. Feedback recipients, in contrast, showed a highly significant Outcomes x Timing interaction, F (1, 116) = 19.6, p < .001, η 2 = .14. Following the feedback conversation, recipients attributed their successes more to internal factors than they did before the conversation and they attributed their failures more to external factors than before ( t (116) = 4.5, p < .001 and t (116) = 3.3, p = .001, respectively). At the end, the two parties’ attributions were well apart on both successes and failures ( t (116) = 2.3, p = .024 and t (116) = 3.0, p = .003). In sum, the performance review discussion led to greater disagreement between the feedback providers and recipients due to the recipients of feedback making more self-enhancing and self-protecting performance attributions.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0234444.g002.jpg

Results are shown by role (provider vs. recipient of feedback), outcomes (successes vs. failures), and timing (before vs. after feedback). Error bars show standard errors.

Performance quality . There were main effects of outcomes and role, but no interactions. As intended, participants rated performance on sales much more highly than they rated the other job aspects (6.72 vs. 3.32 out of 7). Overall, recipients evaluated their performances slightly more positively than the providers did (5.13 vs. 4.91).

Performance importance . There was a main effect of outcome, modified by significant Role x Outcomes and Role x Outcomes x Timing interactions. To understand these effects, we followed up with analyses of role and timing for successes and for failures, separately. Feedback recipients rated their successes as more important than feedback providers did (6.41 and 6.12, respectively; F (1, 115) = 6.20, p = .014, η 2 = .05), with no significant effects of time. In contrast, importance ratings for failures showed a Role x Timing interaction ( F (1, 114) = 7.77, p = .006, η 2 = .06): Providers rated failures as more important before discussion, becoming more lenient following discussion (5.75 vs. 5.42; t (114) = 2.22, p = .028), consistent with the findings of Gioia and Sims [ 32 ]. Recipient ratings showed no significant change as a consequence of discussion.

These analyses suggest that in performance conversations, feedback providers do not lead recipients to see things their way: Recipient interpretations of past performance do not become more like provider interpretations. In fact, following discussion, recipients’ causal attributions are further from those of the providers. Moreover, across dyads, there was no correlation between the recipient’s ratings and the provider’s ratings following discussion: Although a ceiling effect limits the potential for correlations on the quality of sales performance (success), the other measures, especially attributions, show considerable variation in responses across dyads but still no provider-recipient correlations ( S2 and S3 Tables). For performance quality, performance importance, and attributions, for successes and for failures, all | r | < .12 ( p > .22, N = 115 to 117).

Effects of attribution disagreement and future focus on recipients’ acceptance of feedback and intention to change

We hypothesized that provider-recipient disagreement about attributions negatively impacts feedback in two ways, by reducing the extent to which recipients accept the feedback as legitimate, and by reducing the recipient’s intentions to change in response to the feedback. We further hypothesized that a focus on future behavior, rather than past behavior, would engender greater acceptance of feedback and greater intention to change. The present study provides evidence for both of those hypotheses.

We measured feedback acceptance by averaging ratings on feedback accuracy and provider qualifications, both scaled 0 to 100 ( r = .448). We measured intention to change as the average of recipients’ responses to three of the Likert questions in the post-feedback-discussion questionnaire (α = .94):

Based on the feedback, you are now motivated to change your behavior. You see the value of acting on Chris’s suggestions. You will likely change your behavior, based on the feedback received.

We analyzed these two measures of feedback effectiveness using regressions with five variables that might predict the outcome of the discussion: post-feedback disagreement about attributions, performance quality, and performance importance (all scored such that positive numbers indicate that the recipient made judgments more favorable to the recipient than did the provider); how favorable the recipient found the feedback to be (rated from 0 = almost all negative to 10 = almost all positive); and the extent to which the recipient thought the conversation was future focused. This last measure is the average of the recipient’s ratings on the following three Likert questions on the post-feedback questionnaire (α = .75):

You and Chris spent a large part of this session generating new ideas for your next steps. The feedback conversation centered on what will make you most successful going forward. The feedback discussion focused mostly on your future behavior.

We hypothesized that the recipients’ acceptance of feedback and intention to change would be affected by the recipients’ impressions of how future focused the discussion was. That said, we note that the provider’s and the recipient’s ratings of future focus were well correlated across dyads ( r (115) = .423, p < .001), suggesting that recipients’ ratings of future focus reflected characteristics of the discussion that were perceived by both parties.

As shown in Table 3 , recipients’ ratings of future focus proved to be the best predictor of their ratings of both feedback acceptance and intention to change. Recipients’ favorability ratings also significantly predicted their intention to change and, especially, their acceptance of the feedback. Attribution disagreement between providers and recipients predicted lower acceptance of feedback, but not intention to change. Differences of opinion regarding the quality and importance of various aspects of job performance had no significant effects and, as shown by Model 2 in Table 3 , removing them had almost no effect.

Model 1 includes all five predictor variables. Model 2 excludes the two that showed no significant effects in Model 1. Numbers in brackets are adjusted R 2 s.

As in Study 1, we again observe that the providers and recipients of feedback formed very different impressions about past performance. A new and important finding in this study is that feedback conversations did not merely fail to diminish provider-recipient disagreements about what led to strong and weak performance; they actually turned minor disagreements into major ones. Recipients made more self-enhancing and self-protective attributions following the performance discussion, believing more strongly than before that their successes were caused by internal factors (their ability, personality, effort, and attention) and their failures were caused by external factors (job responsibilities, employer expectations, resources provided, and bad luck). There were also modest disagreements regarding the quality and importance of different aspects of the recipient’s job performance, but these did not worsen following discussion. The most important source of disagreement between providers and recipients then, especially following the feedback conversation, was not about what happened, but about why it happened.

What led recipients of performance feedback to accept it as legitimate and helpful? The best predictor of feedback effectiveness was the extent to which the discussion was perceived as future focused. Unsurprisingly, feedback was also easier to accept when it was more favorable. As predicted, recipients were more likely to accept feedback when they and the feedback providers agreed more about what caused the past events. Greater attribution agreement, however, did not increase recipients’ intention to change. These findings suggest that reaching agreement on the causes of past performance is neither likely to happen (because feedback discussions widen causal attribution disagreement) nor is it necessary for fostering change. What does matter is the extent to which the feedback conversation focuses on generating new ideas for future success. We further explore the relations among all these variables following the reporting of Study 3.

Performance feedback serves goals other than improving performance. For example, performance reviews often serve as an opportunity for the feedback provider to justify promotion and compensation decisions. For the recipient, the conversation may provide an opportunity for image management and the chance to influence employment decisions. People may fail to distinguish between evaluation and improvement goals when providing and receiving feedback. In Study 2, the instructions were intended to be explicit in directing participants to the developmental goal of performance improvement, rather than accountability or rewards. Nevertheless, the providers’ wish to justify their evaluations and the recipients’ wish to influence them might have contributed to the differences we observed in attributions and in judgments about the feedback’s legitimacy. To address this concern, we added a page of detailed company guidelines that emphasized the primacy of the performance-improvement goal over the goals of expressing, justifying, or influencing evaluations. There were two versions of these guidelines, which did not differ in their effects.

Participants were 162 executives and MBA students enrolled in advanced Human Resources classes in Australia. An international mix of businesspeople, 74% said they grew up in Australia or New Zealand, 10% in Europe, 22% in Asia, and 7% other. (Totals sum to more than 100% because some participants indicated more than one.) Participants averaged 39 years of age, ranging from 27 to 60. Females comprised 37% of the participants.

Participants read the same scenario and instructions as in Study 2, with an added page of guidelines for giving developmental feedback ( S8 Text ). They then completed the same post-discussion questionnaires used for the pre-post group of Study 2, minus the ratings of performance quality and importance for various aspects of the job, which showed no effects in Study 2. (The full text of the questionnaires is provided in S9 and S10 Texts). Taken together, these modifications kept the procedure to about the same length as in Study 2. This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Melbourne. Written consent was obtained.

Role differences in the interpretation of past performance

As in Study 2, we calculated the sum of the percentages of attributions assigned to internal causes (ability and personality + effort and attention), applying an arcsine transformation. As before, we analyzed the internal attributions measure with a mixed-model ANOVA treating each dyad as a unit. There were two within-dyads variables: role (provider or recipient), and outcomes (successes or failures) and one between-dyads variable (guideline version ). There were no effects involving guideline version (all F < 1). The main effects of role ( F (1, 79) = 50.12, p < .001, η 2 = .39) and outcomes ( F (1, 79) = 113.8, p < .001, η 2 = .59) and the interaction between them ( F (1, 79) = 86.34, p < .001, η 2 = .52) are displayed in Fig 3 , along with the parallel post-feedback results from the previous two studies. As in Study 2, the two parties’ post-discussion attributions were well apart on both successes and, especially, failures ( t (80) = 3.3 and 9.4 respectively, both p ≤ .001). Again, the correlations between the provider’s and the recipient’s post-conversation performance attributions across dyads were not significant for either successes ( r (79) = -.04, p > .69) or failures ( r (79) = -.13, p > .23) suggesting that conversation does not lead the dyad to a common understanding of what led to good or poor performance.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0234444.g003.jpg

Results are shown by role (provider vs. recipient of feedback) and valence/outcomes (positive feedback for successes vs. negative feedback for failures), following feedback conversation. Error bars show standard errors.

We conducted regression analyses of the recipient’s feedback acceptance and intention to change as in Study 2. The regression models included three predictors: future focus, attribution disagreement, and feedback favorability. Results, shown in Table 4 , replicated our Study 2 finding that future focus is the best predictor of both feedback acceptance and intention to change. As before, attribution disagreement predicted lower acceptance, but in this study it also predicted less intention to change. We again found that feedback favorability ratings were associated with greater acceptance, but this time, not with intention to change. Recipients and providers were again significantly correlated in their judgments of how future focused the conversation was ( r (79) = .299, p = .007).

Numbers in brackets are adjusted R 2 s.

Future focus, as perceived by the recipients of feedback, was once again the strongest predictor of their acceptance of the feedback and the strongest predictor of their intention to change. Conversely, attribution disagreement between the provider and recipient of feedback was associated with lower feedback acceptance and weaker intention to change. As in Studies 1 and 2, recipients made more internal attributions for successes than providers did and, especially, more external attributions for failures. The added guidelines in this study emphasizing performance-improvement goals over evaluative ones did not alleviate provider-recipient attribution differences. Indeed, those differences were considerably larger in this study than in the previous one and were more similar to those seen in Study 1 (see Fig 3 ).

Future focus, attributions, favorability, and the effectiveness of feedback

The strongest predictor of feedback effectiveness is the recipient’s perception that the feedback conversation focused on plans for the future rather than analysis of the past. We seek here to elucidate the relationship between future focus and feedback effectiveness by looking at the interrelations among the three predictors of effectiveness we studied: future focus, attribution disagreement, and feedback favorability.

The analyses that follow include data from all participants who were asked for ratings of future focus, namely those in Study 3 and in the pre-post group of Study 2. We included study as a variable in our analyses; no effects involving the study variable were significant. Nonetheless, because the two studies drew from different samples and used slightly different methods, inferential statistics could be impacted by intraclass correlation within each study. Therefore, we also tested for study-specific differences in parameter estimates using hierarchical linear modeling [ 58 , 59 ]. No significant differences between studies emerged, confirming the appropriateness of combining the data. (The HLM results are provided in S2 Analyses .)

The association between future focus and feedback effectiveness could be mediated by the effects of attribution disagreement and/or feedback favorability. Specifically, it could be that perceiving the conversation as more future focused is associated with closer agreement on attributions or with perceiving the feedback as more favorable, and one or both of those latter two effects leads to improved feedback effectiveness. Tests of mediation, following the methods of Kenny and colleagues [ 60 ], suggest otherwise (see Fig 4 ). These analyses partition the total associations of future focus with feedback acceptance and with intention to change into direct effects and indirect effects. Indirect effects via reduced attribution disagreement were 6.2% of the relation of future focus to feedback acceptance and 2.2% to intention to change. Indirect effects via improved perceptions of feedback favorability were 20.8% of the relation of future focus to feedback acceptance and 4.5% to intention to change. Thus, there is little to suggest that closer agreement on attributions or improved perceptions of feedback favorability account for the benefits of future focus on feedback effectiveness.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0234444.g004.jpg

The two feedback effectiveness measures are feedback acceptance and intention to change. Following Kenny (2018), standardized regression coefficients are shown for the relations between future focus and two hypothesized mediators, attribution disagreement and feedback favorability ( a ), the mediators and the feedback effectiveness measures controlling for future focus ( b ), future focus and the effectiveness measures ( c ), and future focus and the effectiveness measures controlling for the mediator ( c′ ). The total effect ( c ) equals the direct effect ( c′ ) plus the indirect effect ( a · b ). Data are from Studies 2 and 3. a p = .072; * p = .028; ** p < .001.

Interactions

Future focus might have synergistic or moderating effects. In particular, we hypothesized that perceiving the conversation as more future focused may moderate the negative impact of attribution disagreement on feedback effectiveness. Alternatively, future focus may be especially beneficial when agreement about attributions is good, or when attribution differences are neither so big that they cannot be put aside, nor so small that the parties see eye to eye even when they focus on the past. Similarly, future focus may be especially beneficial when feedback is most unfavorable to the recipient, or when it’s most favorable, or when it is neither so negative that the recipients can’t move past it, nor so positive that the recipients accept it even when the conversation focuses on the past.

We conducted regression analyses with feedback acceptance and intention to change as dependent variables and future focus, feedback favorability, attribution disagreement, and their first-order interactions as predictors. Because some plausible interactions are nonlinear, we defined low, intermediate, and high values for each of the three predictor variables, dividing the 198 participants as evenly as possible for each. We then partitioned each predictor into linear and quadratic components with one degree of freedom each. With linear and quadratic components of three predictors plus a binary variable for Study 2 vs. Study 3, there were seven potential linear effects and 18 possible two-way interactions. We used a stepwise procedure to select which interactions to include in our regressions, using an inclusion parameter of p < .15. Results are shown in Table 5 .

Models include all main effects and those first-order interactions that met an entry criterion of p < .15, plus data source (Study 2 vs. Study 3). Statistically significant values are underlined.

Future focus interacted with feedback favorability—marginally for feedback acceptance and significantly for intention to change. As shown in Fig 5 , recipients who gave low or intermediate ratings for future focus accepted the feedback less when it was most negative ( t (128) = 5.21, p < .001) and similarly, reported less inclination to change ( t (128) = 3.23, p = .002). In contrast, the recipients who rated the feedback discussion as most future focused accepted their feedback and indicated high intention to change at all levels of feedback favorability. These patterns suggest that perceiving future focus moderates the deleterious effect of negative feedback on feedback effectiveness.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0234444.g005.jpg

Results for each measure of feedback effectiveness are shown by three levels of perceived future focus and three levels of perceived feedback favorability. Error bars show standard errors. Data are from Studies 2 and 3.

On the other hand, we find no evidence that future focus moderates the negative effect of attribution disagreement on feedback effectiveness. Future focus did interact marginally with attribution disagreement for intention to change. However, the benefits of perceiving high vs. low future focus may, in fact, be stronger when there is closer agreement about attributions: The increase in intention to change between low and high future focus groups was 2.30 with high disagreement, 2.37 with intermediate disagreement, and 3.24 in dyads with low disagreement, on a scale from 1 to 7.

Regression-tree analyses

Regression-tree analyses can provide additional insights into the non-linear relations among variables [ 61 ], with a better visualization of the best and worst conditions to facilitate feedback acceptance and intention to change. These analyses use the predictors (here, future focus, attribution disagreement, and feedback favorability) to divide participants into subgroups empirically, maximizing the extent to which values on the dependent measure are homogeneous within subgroups and different between them. We generated regression trees for each of our two effectiveness measures, feedback acceptance and intention to change. Fig 6 shows the results, including all subgroups (nodes) with N = 10 or more.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is pone.0234444.g006.jpg

The trees depict the effects of future focus, attribution disagreement, and feedback favorability on our two measures of feedback effectiveness. The width of branches is proportional to the number of participants in that branch. Node 0 is the full sample of 198. Values on the X axis are standardized values for each dependent measure. Data are from Studies 2 and 3.

Both trees show that future focus is the most important variable, dividing into lower and higher branches at Nodes 1 and 2, and further distinguishing highest-future groups at Nodes A8 and B6. These representations also reinforce the conclusion that perceived future focus does not operate mainly via an association with more positive feedback or with better agreement on attributions. However, attribution disagreement does play a role, with more agreement leading to better acceptance of feedback and greater intention to change, as long as future focus is at least moderately high (Nodes A3 vs. A4 and B7 vs. B8). (The lack of effect at Node B6 is likely a ceiling effect.) Unfavorable feedback makes matters worse under adverse conditions: when future focus is low (Nodes B3 vs. B4) or when future focus is moderate but attribution disagreement is large (nodes A5 vs. A6).

General discussion

Our research was motivated by a need to understand why performance feedback conversations do not benefit performance to the extent intended and what might be done to improve that situation. We investigated how providers and recipients of workplace feedback differ in their judgements about the causes of performance and the credibility of feedback, and how feedback discussions impact provider-recipient (dis)agreement and feedback effectiveness. We were particularly interested in how interpretations of past performance, feedback acceptance, and intention to change are affected by the recipient’s perception of temporal focus, that is, the extent to which the feedback discussion focuses on past versus future behavior.

Management theorists typically advocate evaluating performance relative to established goals and standards, diagnosing the causes of substandard performance, and providing feedback so that people can learn from the past [ 19 ]. They also posit that feedback recipients must recognize there is a problem, accept the feedback as accurate, and find the feedback providers fair and credible in order for performance feedback to motivate improvement [ 7 , 14 , 35 ]. Unfortunately, we know that performance feedback often does not motivate improvement [ 4 ]. Our research contributes in several ways to understanding why that is and how feedback conversations might be made more effective.

Decades of attribution theory and research have elucidated the biases thought to produce discrepant explanations for performance between the providers and recipients of feedback. We show that for negative feedback, these discrepancies are prevalent in the workplace. We also show that larger attribution discrepancies are associated with greater rejection of feedback and, in our performance review simulations, with weaker intention to change. These findings support recent research and theory linking performance feedback, work-related decision making, and attribution theory: Instead of changing behavior in response to mixed or negative feedback, people make self-enhancing and self-protecting attributions and judgements they can use to justify not changing [ 8 , 14 , 62 ].

Our research suggests that the common practice of discussing the employees’ past performance, with an emphasis on how and why outcomes occurred and what that implies about the employees’ strengths and weaknesses, can be counterproductive. Although the parties to a feedback discussion may agree reasonably well about which goals and standards were met or unmet, they are unlikely to converge on an understanding of the causes of unmet goals and standards, even with engaged give and take. Instead, the feedback conversation creates or exacerbates disagreement about the causes of performance outcomes, leading feedback recipients to take more credit for their successes and less responsibility for their failures. This suggests that feedback conversations that attempt to diagnose past performance act as another form of self-threat that increases the self-serving bias [ 33 ]. Surely this runs counter to what the feedback provider intended.

At the same time, we find that self-serving attributions need not stand in the way of feedback acceptance and motivation to improve. A key discovery in our research is that the more recipients feel the feedback focuses on next steps and future actions, the more they accept the feedback and the more they intend to act on it. In fact, when feedback is perceived to be highly future focused, feedback recipients respond as well to predominantly negative feedback as to predominantly positive feedback. Future focus does not nullify self-serving attributions and their detrimental effects [see also 63 ], but it does enable productive feedback discussions despite them.

We used two complementary research methods. Study 1 used a more naturalistic and thus more ecologically valid method, collecting retrospective self-reports from hundreds of managers about actual feedback interactions in a wide variety of work situations [see 64 ]. Studies 2 and 3 used a role-play method that allowed us to give all participants identical workplace performance information, a good portion of which was undisputed and quantitative. With that design, response differences between the providers and recipients of feedback are due entirely to role, unconfounded by differences in knowledge and experience.

What role plays cannot establish is the magnitude of effects in organizational settings. Attribution misalignment and resistance to feedback might easily be much stronger in real workplace performance reviews where it would be rare for the parties to arrive with identical, largely unambiguous information. Moreover, managers’ investment in the monetary and career outcomes of performance reviews might lead feedback recipients to feel more threatened than in a role play and thus to disagree even more with unfavorable feedback. On the other hand, the desire to maintain employment and/or to maintain good relationships with supervisors might motivate managers to re-assess their past achievements, to change their private attributions, and to be more accepting of unfavorable feedback. Data from our role-play studies may not speak to the magnitude of resistance to feedback in work settings (although our survey results suggest it’s substantial), but they do show that feedback acceptance is increased when the participants perceive their feedback to be focused on the future.

Implications for future research and theory

There are few research topics more important to the study of organizations than performance management. Feedback conversations are a cornerstone of most individual and team performance management, yet there is still much we do not know about what should be said, how, and why. Based on research into the motivational advantages of prospective thinking, we hypothesized that feedback discussions perceived as future focused are the most effective kind for generating acceptance of feedback and fostering positive behavior change. Our findings support that hypothesis. The present research contributes to the literature on prospection by highlighting the role of interpersonal interactions in facilitating prefactual thinking and any associated advantages for goal pursuit [ 39 , 43 – 45 , 63 , 65 ]. In this section we suggest three lines of future research: (a) field studies and interventions; (b) research into the potential role of self-beliefs; and (c) exploration of the conversational dynamics associated with feedback perceived as past vs. future focused.

Field research and intervention designs

Testing feedback interventions in the workplace and other field settings is an important future step toward corroborating, elaborating, or correcting our findings. It will be necessary to develop effective means to foster a more future-focused style of feedback. Then, randomized controlled trials that contrast future-focused with diagnostic feedback can demonstrate the benefits that may accrue from focusing feedback more on future behavior and less on past behavior. Participant evaluations of the feedback discussions can be supplemented by those of neutral observers. Such evaluations are directly relevant to organizational goals, including employee motivation, positive supervisor-supervisee relations, and effective problem solving. Assessing subsequent behavior change and job performance is both important and complicated for evaluating feedback effectiveness: Seeing intentions through to fruition depends on many factors, including individual differences in self-regulation [ 66 , 67 ] and factors beyond people’s control, such as competing commitments, limited resources, and changing priorities [ 68 – 71 ]. Nevertheless, the ultimate proof of future-focused feedback will lie in performance improvement itself.

Self-beliefs and future focus

If future focus enhances feedback effectiveness, it may do so via self-beliefs. Growth mindset and self-efficacy, for example, are self-beliefs that influence how people think about and act on the future. Discussions that focus on what people can do in the future to improve performance may encourage people to view their own behavior as malleable and to view better results as achievable. If future focus helps people access this growth mindset, it should orient them toward mastering challenges and improving the self for the future: Whereas people exercise defensive self-esteem repair when in a fixed mindset, they prefer self-improvement when accessing a growth mindset [ 72 , 73 ]. Similarly, feedback conversations that focus on ways the feedback recipient can attain goals in the future may enhance people’s confidence in their ability to execute the appropriate strategies and necessary behaviors to succeed. Such self-efficacy expectancies have been shown to influence the goals people select, the effort and resources they devote, their persistence in the face of obstacles, and the motivation to get started [ 74 , 75 ]. Thus, research is needed to assess whether future focus alters people’s self-beliefs (or vice versa; see below) and if these, in turn, impact people’s acceptance of feedback and intention to change.

We found sizeable variation in the extent to which dyads reported focusing on the future. Pre-existing individual differences in self-beliefs may contribute to that variation. Recent research, for example, finds that professors with more growth mindsets have students who perform better and report being more motivated to do their best work [ 76 ]. In the case of a feedback conversation, we suspect that either party can initiate thinking prospectively, but both must participate in it to sustain the benefits.

Conversational dynamics and future focus

Unlike most studies of people’s reactions to mixed or negative feedback, our studies use face-to-face, real-time interaction, that is to say, two people in conversation. Might conversational dynamics associated with future-focused feedback contribute to its being better accepted and more motivating than feedback focused on the past? Do managers who focus more on the future listen to other people’s ideas and perspectives in ways that are perceived as more empathic and nonjudgmental? Do these more prospective discussions elicit greater cooperative problem solving? Research on conversation in the workplace is in its early stages [ 77 ], but some studies support the idea that high quality listening and partner responsiveness might reduce defensiveness, increase self-awareness, or produce greater willingness to consider new perspectives and ideas [ 78 , 79 ].

Practical implications

Our studies provide the first empirical evidence that managers can make feedback more effective by focusing it on the future. Future-focused feedback, as we define it, is characterized by prospective thinking and by collaboration in generating ideas, planning, and problem-solving. We assessed the degree of future focus by asking participants to rate the extent to which the feedback discussion focused on future behavior, the two parties spent time generating new ideas for next steps, and the conversation centered on how to make the recipient successful. This differs greatly from feedback research that distinguishes past vs. future orientation “using minimal rewording of each critique comment” (e.g., you didn’t always demonstrate awareness of… vs. you should aim to demonstrate more awareness of…) [ 80 p. 1866].

Because future-focused feedback is feedback, it also differs from both advice giving and “feedforward” (although it might be advantageous to incorporate these): It differs from Kluger and Nir’s feedforward interview, which queries how the conditions that enabled a person’s positive work experiences might be replicated in the future [ 81 ], and from Goldsmith’s feedforward exercise, which involves requesting and receiving suggestions for the future, without discussion or feedback [ 82 ].

The scenario at the very start of this article asks, “What can Chris say to get through to Taylor?” A future-focused answer might include the following: Chris first clarifies that the purpose of the feedback is to improve Taylor’s future performance, with the goal of furthering Taylor’s career. Chris applauds Taylor’s successes and is forthright and specific about Taylor’s shortcomings, while avoiding discussion of causes and explanations. Chris signals belief that Taylor has the motivation and competence to improve [ 83 ]. Chris then initiates a discussion in which they work together to develop ideas for how Taylor can achieve better outcomes in the future. (For a more detailed illustration of a future-focused conversation, see S11 Text .)

Conclusions

Our research supports the intriguing possibility that the future of feedback could be more effective and less aversive than its past. Performance management need not be tied to unearthing the determinants of past performance and holding people to account for past failures. Rather, performance may be managed most successfully by collaborating with the feedback recipient to generate next steps, to develop opportunities for interesting and worthwhile endeavors, and to enlarge the vision of what the recipient could accomplish. Most organizations and most managers want their workers to perform well. Most workers wish to succeed at their jobs. Everyone benefits when feedback discussions develop new ideas and solutions and when the recipients of feedback are motivated to make changes based on those. A future-focused approach to feedback holds great promise for motivating future performance improvement.

Supporting information

S1 analyses, s2 analyses, acknowledgments.

For helpful comments on earlier drafts of this paper, we are grateful to Pino Audia, Angelo Denisi, Nick Epley, Ayelet Fishbach, Brian Gibbs, Reid Hastie, Chris Hsee, Remus Ilies, David Nussbaum, Jay Russo, Paul Schoemaker, William Swann, and Kathleen Vohs.

Funding Statement

This research received funding from the Melbourne Business School while the first three authors were either visiting (JG, JK) or permanent (IOW) faculty there. While working on this research, the first two authors (JG, JK) also worked as owners and employees of management consulting firm Humanly Possible. Humanly Possible provided support in the form of salaries and profit-sharing compensation for authors JG and JK, but did not have any additional role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. The specific roles of these authors are articulated in the “author contributions” section.

Data Availability

  • PLoS One. 2020; 15(6): e0234444.

Decision Letter 0

PONE-D-20-05644

The future of feedback:  Motivating performance improvement

Dear Dr Klayman,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by May 22 2020 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.
  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.
  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Paola Iannello

Academic Editor

Journal requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements:

1.    Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at http://www.plosone.org/attachments/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and http://www.plosone.org/attachments/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please modify the title to ensure that it is meeting PLOS’ guidelines ( https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-title ). In particular, the title should be "specific, descriptive, concise, and comprehensible to readers outside the field" and in this case it is not informative and specific about your study's scope and methodology.

3. Thank you for stating the following in the Competing Interests section:

"The authors have declared that no competing interests exist."

We note that one or more of the authors are employed by a commercial company: Humanly Possible, Inc.

1.     Please provide an amended Funding Statement declaring this commercial affiliation, as well as a statement regarding the Role of Funders in your study. If the funding organization did not play a role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript and only provided financial support in the form of authors' salaries and/or research materials, please review your statements relating to the author contributions, and ensure you have specifically and accurately indicated the role(s) that these authors had in your study. You can update author roles in the Author Contributions section of the online submission form.

Please also include the following statement within your amended Funding Statement.

“The funder provided support in the form of salaries for authors [insert relevant initials], but did not have any additional role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. The specific roles of these authors are articulated in the ‘author contributions’ section.”

If your commercial affiliation did play a role in your study, please state and explain this role within your updated Funding Statement.

2. Please also provide an updated Competing Interests Statement declaring this commercial affiliation along with any other relevant declarations relating to employment, consultancy, patents, products in development, or marketed products, etc.  

Within your Competing Interests Statement, please confirm that this commercial affiliation does not alter your adherence to all PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials by including the following statement: "This does not alter our adherence to  PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials.” (as detailed online in our guide for authors http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/competing-interests ) . If this adherence statement is not accurate and  there are restrictions on sharing of data and/or materials, please state these. Please note that we cannot proceed with consideration of your article until this information has been declared.

Please include both an updated Funding Statement and Competing Interests Statement in your cover letter. We will change the online submission form on your behalf.

Please know it is PLOS ONE policy for corresponding authors to declare, on behalf of all authors, all potential competing interests for the purposes of transparency. PLOS defines a competing interest as anything that interferes with, or could reasonably be perceived as interfering with, the full and objective presentation, peer review, editorial decision-making, or publication of research or non-research articles submitted to one of the journals. Competing interests can be financial or non-financial, professional, or personal. Competing interests can arise in relationship to an organization or another person. Please follow this link to our website for more details on competing interests: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/competing-interests

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: 1. I enjoyed reading this manuscript, but it appears to be unnecessary long in parts and readability would benefit of a more concise style. I would recommend condensing some parts, for example in the methods section for study 2 was overly long and lacked clarity in parts. The description of the second questionnaire was a little confusing in terms of the consistency in how items were measured and the hypothesis was not clear.

2. In the ethics statement for Study 1 (line 184), please explain the rationale behind the waiver of consent.

3. Procedure (line 187) please give details of the survey platform used.

4. Results -Please include the number of participants in each group.

5. Please comment on what normality checks were performed to assess the distribution of the data.

6. Line 470, correlations are discussed but I can’t see a table to support these.

7. The discussion did not address the results in relation to previous literature and lacked a theoretical explanation of the findings (See for example ‘Korn CW, Rosenblau G, Rodriguez Buritica JM, Heekeren HR (2016) Performance Feedback Processing Is Positively Biased As Predicted by Attribution Theory. PLoS ONE 11(2)’ for a discussion of attributional style and self-serving bias. I recommend some rewrite of the discussion with more reference to theory.

8. Some acknowledgement of the effect of individual differences in self-regulation would be useful to include as this may influence how feedback is received in terms of attributions. See for example, ‘Donovan, JJ, Lorenzet, SJ, Dwight, SA, Schneider, D. The impact of goal progress and individual differences on self‐regulation in training. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2018; 48: 661– 674’.

9. The suggestions for improvement at the end of the study would be better to be condensed to give a brief suggestion of methods.

Reviewer #2: The paper reports an interesting and comprehensive work about a relevant issue in organizational psychology. Both the theoretical frame and the applied methodology are original and thorough, though the use of role-play raises some doubts about the robustness of the results (some concerns are raised by the authors themselves (lines 752-760) ). This is, in my opinion, the main limitation of studies 2 and 3. I would suggest that the authors insert a wider reasoning about the choice of using this method to collect their data and the pros and cons.

In the "General Discussion" paragraph the authors state that "We investigated the sources of agreement and disagreement between feedback provider and recipient" (lines 712-713). I strongly suggest that this sentence is being modified, since it doesn't describe the aim nor the results in Study 1 correctly.

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article ( what does this mean? ). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy .

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes: Federica Biassoni

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/ . PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at gro.solp@serugif . Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Author response to Decision Letter 0

12 May 2020

Please see uploaded document Response to Reviewers. Text copied here.

Response to Reviewers

We wish to thank the reviewers for their very helpful and constructive comments. We especially appreciate the clarity and specificity with which they framed their suggestions. Below we respond to each reviewer recommendation.

Reviewer #1:

1. I enjoyed reading this manuscript, but it appears to be unnecessary long in parts and readability would benefit of a more concise style. I would recommend condensing some parts, for example in the methods section for study 2 was overly long and lacked clarity in parts. The description of the second questionnaire was a little confusing in terms of the consistency in how items were measured and the hypothesis was not clear.

We revised the methods section for Study 2 (former lines 274-279; 285-414, revision lines 276-281; 299-402). The new version is a full page shorter and, in line with the reviewer’s suggestion, we believe this more concise version is now more readable. It includes a revised description of the post-discussion questionnaires (former 346-367; revision 350-361), clarifying the sequence and types of questions provided to each group. It also includes revisions, mainly in the Design section (former 387-414; revision lines 377-402) to clarify how the various measures related to our hypotheses.

Study 1 was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of Chicago, which waived the requirement for written consent as was its customary policy for studies judged to be minimal risk, involving only individual, anonymized survey responses. Their decision cited US Code 45 CFR 46.101(b). Citing the code in our manuscript seemed overly legalistic, but we have added the rest of the rationale to the ethics statement (former lines 184-185; revision 184-186).

We now identify the platform as Cogix ViewsFlash (revision line 188).

We have added the requested information for Study 1 (revision lines 214-215). Following up on the suggestion, we also made it easier to locate the corresponding information for Study 2 (revision lines 316-317).

The general consensus is that the analyses we use, i.e. ANOVA and linear regression, are generally quite robust with regard to moderate violations of normality with Ns on the order of ours (e.g., Blanca, Alarcón, Arnau, Bono, & Bendayan, Psichothema, 2017; Schmidt & Finana, Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 2018; Ali & Sharma, Journal of Econometrics, 1996; Schmider, Ziegler Danay, Beyer, & Bühner, Methodology, 2010). Nevertheless, we used an arcsine transformation on the variables a priori most likely to suffer from systematic deviations, namely the attribution proportions. Most authors recommend checking for major deviations from normality by plotting model-predicted values against residuals and against the normal distribution (using P-P or Q-Q plots). We did that for our analyses (graphs attached), and found no troublesome deviations, with the possible exception of one variable of minor importance to our main results or theory, namely performance quality ratings for successes in Study 2. We note in the paper that that variable may suffer from ceiling effects (former 468-469, revision 456-457). We did not add a discussion of normality to the paper because of the increased length and complexity that would involve and because it’s seldom an issue of concern with data and analyses like ours. However, we could include the graphs we’ve attached here as supplemental material if you tell us you would like us to do so.

Thank you for alerting us to this inadvertent omission. We now include complete correlation tables for all the variables analyzed in each Study in the supplemental materials: S2 Table for Study 1 (revision lines 224-225) and S11 Tables for Studies 2 and 3 separately and combined (revision lines 458-459), with provider-recipient correlations identified by color shading. (S2 was formerly the dataset for Study 1, but now data from all three studies are contained in S17.)

To better address our results in relation to previous attribution literature and theory, we have revised former lines 723-740 in the General Discussion. Now we more clearly discuss our findings in relation to self-serving bias, self-threat, and both historical and more recent formulations of attribution theory, including the helpful reference the reviewer provided (revision lines 708-735). We have also added a brief discussion of how our results relate to previous literature on future thinking (revision lines 760-762). We attempted to minimize redundancy with the Introduction section. The new material includes several new references.

We added mention in the General Discussion of individual differences in self-regulation, citing two references, including the one helpfully provided by Reviewer #1 (revision line 776). Additionally, we reworded former lines 798-799 (revision lines 793-794) to make it clearer that we are acknowledging individual differences there as well.

We condensed former lines 828-846 from 19 lines to 8 lines (revision lines 823-830), referring the interested reader to new Supporting Information S16 Text for the expanded version. We trust this solution meets the recommendation for a brief suggestion of methods, while also satisfying the interests of those seeking more detail.

Reviewer #2:

1. The paper reports an interesting and comprehensive work about a relevant issue in organizational psychology. Both the theoretical frame and the applied methodology are original and thorough, though the use of role-play raises some doubts about the robustness of the results (some concerns are raised by the authors themselves (lines 752-760)). This is, in my opinion, the main limitation of studies 2 and 3. I would suggest that the authors insert a wider reasoning about the choice of using this method to collect their data and the pros and cons.

We now include a wider reasoning about our choice to use a role-play method and the pros and cons. The new version comprises revision lines 282-298. (We also revised the subsequent paragraph for increased clarity, given the insertion of the new paragraph about the role-play method.)

2. In the "General Discussion" paragraph the authors state that "We investigated the sources of agreement and disagreement between feedback provider and recipient" (lines 712-713). I strongly suggest that this sentence is being modified, since it doesn't describe the aim nor the results in Study 1 correctly.

Thank you for your careful reading. We have re-written that sentence to more accurately capture the results of Study 1 as well as the other two studies (revised lines 697-700).

[Figures attached--please see uploaded document Response to Reviewers.]

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

27 May 2020

The future of feedback: Survey and role-play investigations into causal attributions, feedback acceptance, motivation to improve, and the potential benefits of future focus for increasing feedback effectiveness in the workplace

PONE-D-20-05644R1

Dear Dr. Klayman,

We are pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it complies with all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you will receive an e-mail containing information on the amendments required prior to publication. When all required modifications have been addressed, you will receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will proceed to our production department and be scheduled for publication.

Shortly after the formal acceptance letter is sent, an invoice for payment will follow. To ensure an efficient production and billing process, please log into Editorial Manager at https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ , click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at gro.solp@gnillibrohtua .

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to enable them to help maximize its impact. If they will be preparing press materials for this manuscript, you must inform our press team as soon as possible and no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact gro.solp@sserpeno .

With kind regards,

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

6. Review Comments to the Author

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article ( what does this mean? ). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

Acceptance letter

The future of feedback:  Motivating performance improvement through future-focused feedback 

Dear Dr. Klayman:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact gro.solp@sserpeno .

If we can help with anything else, please email us at gro.solp@enosolp .

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Paola Iannello

IMAGES

  1. Free Feedback Form Templates

    customer feedback research paper

  2. Questionnaire for Customer Satisfaction

    customer feedback research paper

  3. FREE 13+ Sample Customer Feedback Forms in MS Word

    customer feedback research paper

  4. FREE 9+ Feedback Questionnaire Examples & Samples in PDF

    customer feedback research paper

  5. Customer Feedback Questionnaire Template MS Word

    customer feedback research paper

  6. Customer Research Template

    customer feedback research paper

VIDEO

  1. Qualitative feedback: Insights on Feedback, User Experience, and Customer Satisfaction

  2. ITALO DISCO NEW GENERATION OCTUBRE 2023

  3. Lecture 25: Assessing Leadership Behaviors: Multi-rater feedback instruments

  4. Response to Reviewers for Resubmitting a Paper: Tips for Graduate Students

  5. ITALO DISCO & HIGH NRG NOV 2023

  6. Anticipate Feedback: Turn Criticism into Your Ally #irfannawaz #phd #research #tips

COMMENTS

  1. Exploring The Impact of Customer Feedback on the Well-Being of Service Entities: A TSR Perspective

    Purpose - Adopting the transformative service research (TSR) perspective, the purpose of this paper is to investigate the impact of positive customer feedback on the well-being of front-line ...

  2. Understanding the link between customer feedback metrics and firm

    Practitioners utilise customer feedback metrics (CFM's) to monitor business performance. However, the influence of CFM's on firm performance has been ignored. Thus, this paper aims to examine the effects of CFM's on firm performance. Our study collected data about CFM's, marketing efforts, and financial performance over the period 2005-2020 ...

  3. [PDF] Exploring the impact of customer feedback on the well-being of

    Purpose - Adopting the transformative service research (TSR) perspective, the purpose of this paper is to investigate the impact of positive customer feedback on the well-being of front-line employees, companies, and society. Using a multidisciplinary approach, the overlooked area of "positive feedback" is explored resulting in the development of the "Positive Feedback Model" (PFM).

  4. Customer satisfaction, loyalty behaviors, and firm financial

    The authors synthesize research on the relationship of customer satisfaction with customer- and firm-level outcomes using a meta-analysis based on 535 correlations from 245 articles representing a combined sample size of 1,160,982. The results show a positive association of customer satisfaction with customer-level outcomes (retention, WOM, spending, and price) and firm-level outcomes (product ...

  5. Artificial Intelligence applications to customer feedback research: A

    In this paper, we aim to provide a comprehensive overview of customer feedback literature, highlighting the burgeoning role of AI. Customer feedback has long be ... We begin by touching upon conventional customer feedback research and chart its evolution through the years as the nature of available data and analysis tools develop. We conclude ...

  6. Customer Engagement: A Systematic Review and Future Research Priorities

    This is concerning, as a lack of alignment may result in misinterpretations, causing further divergence in future research. This paper thus offers a systematic review of the extant literature on customer engagement dated from 2009-2018, reflecting: (i) leading conceptualisations and manifestations of CE, (ii) customer- and firm-related CE ...

  7. Full article: Customer satisfaction, loyalty, knowledge and

    Therefore, the subject of this paper is customer satisfaction, loyalty, knowledge and business competitiveness from the perspective of a food-industry customer. ... of customer satisfaction is wider and falls within the definition of satisfaction as it was understand in this research. Customer loyalty is defined by Oliver (Citation 1997) ...

  8. Customer experience: a systematic literature review and consumer

    The existing state of customer experience research was assessed by reviewing 99 articles. Table 2 reveals that the customer experience has been studied in four categories; with most of the articles published in the context of experience with a brand (n = 35), followed by the context of experience with a product/service (n = 28), experience with a website or a specific medium (n = 19), and the ...

  9. Artificial Intelligence Applications to Customer Feedback Research: A

    Abstract. In this paper, we aim to provide a comprehensive overview of customer feedback literature, highlighting the burgeoning role of artificial intelligence (AI). Customer feedback has long been a valuable source of customer insights for businesses and market researchers. While previously survey focused, customer feedback in the digital age ...

  10. Customer feedback gathering and management tools for product-service

    Towards that, this research work presents tools for customers' feedback gathering and management to support efficient and accurate product and service designs. Mobile applications for customer feedback gathering on products and services and an importance-satisfaction analysis based on modular design approach are proposed.

  11. (PDF) Exploring the Impact of Customer Feedback on the Well-Being of

    Positive customer feedback is a highly under-researched topic. Most previous research has approached customer feedback from a negative lens, focusing on the impact of 2 customer complaints, dysfunctional customer behavior and service recovery, on front-line employees and companies in general.

  12. Customer Satisfaction: Articles, Research, & Case Studies on Customer

    New research on customer satisfaction from Harvard Business School faculty on issues such as the distinction between understanding and listening to customers, how to determine how much of a CEO's time should be spent interacting with customers, and how satisfied employees and customers can drive lifelong profit. ... A new working paper, "To ...

  13. Impact of Service Quality on the Customer Satisfaction: Case ...

    The purpose of this project was to determine the Impact of technical and functional service quality on the customer satisfaction and loyalty. Though conducting a survey number of results was collected, and the results belong to their view about different types of platform which they used for online meeting and academic stuff.

  14. A Research Paper on the Effects of Customer Feedback on Business

    Impact Factor 6.252. A Research Paper on the Effects of Customer. Feedback on Business. Zeel Shah and Shambhu Rai 2. Student 1 and Professor. Bharati Vidyapeeth's Institute of Management and ...

  15. A Study on the Factors Affecting Customer Satisfaction with

    2.1. Research Design. This study analyzed customer satisfaction factors by taking a user-based approach and using an exploratory sequential mixed method [19,20].The qualitative research method was used because it is suitable for collecting information about issues and problems in the field, focusing on the perspectives and views of various research participants, and understanding and ...

  16. The future of feedback: Motivating performance improvement through

    Indirect effects via reduced attribution disagreement were 6.2% of the relation of future focus to feedback acceptance and 2.2% to intention to change. Indirect effects via improved perceptions of feedback favorability were 20.8% of the relation of future focus to feedback acceptance and 4.5% to intention to change.

  17. PDF CUSTOMER SATISFACTION AND CUSTOMER COMPLAINTS

    tomer complaints, strategies to collect customer feedback, steps in handling customer complaints. Also, the concept of customer satisfaction was discussed, the importance of customer satisfaction, how to measure and achieve customer satisfaction. Both the qualitative and the quantitative methods were used in this research work. Questionnaires

  18. (PDF) An empirical research on customer satisfaction study: a

    The contribution of this paper is to propose two satisfaction levels of CSI models for analyzing customer satisfaction and loyalty, thereby helping tourism factory managers improve customer ...

  19. A Research Paper on the Effects of Customer Feedback on Business

    A Research Paper on the Effects of Customer Feedback on Business. Zeel Shah, Shambhu Rai. Published in International Journal of… 11 July 2022. Business. Many businesses have struggled for years to keep their existing consumers while simultaneously gaining new ones. In this aspect, businesses have been able to analyse and upgrade their ...

  20. Impact of product quality on customer satisfaction: A Systematic

    [email protected]. ABSTRACT. This research explains a conceptual model that elucidates the factors. that in uence the impact of product quality on customer satisfac-. tion and how this ...