Advertisement

Advertisement

Does Sex Really Sell? Paradoxical Effects of Sexualization in Advertising on Product Attractiveness and Purchase Intentions

  • Original Article
  • Open access
  • Published: 23 September 2020
  • Volume 84 , pages 701–719, ( 2021 )

Cite this article

You have full access to this open access article

sex sells essay

  • Sarah Gramazio 1 ,
  • Mara Cadinu 1 ,
  • Francesca Guizzo   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-0754-3337 1 &
  • Andrea Carnaghi 2  

62k Accesses

18 Citations

111 Altmetric

Explore all metrics

To test the “sex sells” assumption, we examined how Italian men and women react to sexualized advertising. Women showed lower product attractiveness and purchase intentions toward products presented with sexualized female models than with neutral ads, whereas men were unaffected by ads’ sexualization (Study 1, n  = 251). Study 2 ( n  = 197) replicated the overall results. Study 3 ( n  = 198) tested hostile sexism as a moderator as well as negative emotions as a mediator of consumers’ responses. Especially men with higher hostile sexism showed more purchase intentions after viewing female sexualized ads than neutral ads. Moreover, women’s lower consumer responses toward sexualized female ads were due to higher negative emotions. Study 4 ( n  = 207) included ads with both female and male models, replicating responses to female sexualization and showing that both women and men had lower product attractiveness and purchase intentions toward male sexualized ads than neutral ads. Replicating and extending Study 3’s results, women’s negative emotions was the mediator. The present study has practical implications for marketers because it suggests that “sex does not sell.” In addition, considering both the psychological damage and practical inefficacy of sexualized ads, our findings have important implications for public policy.

Similar content being viewed by others

sex sells essay

The Role of Female Sexual Self-Schema in Reactions to Non-explicit Sexual Advertising Imagery

Do sexy mouthwash ads leave a bad aftertaste the interaction of sexual self-schema and brand positioning fit on female-targeted sexual advertising efficacy.

sex sells essay

Does “Hot” Lead to “Not So Hot?” Sexy Images, Indulgent Consumption, and the Impacts of Gender and Self-Construal: An Abstract

Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.

At the Cannes Lions Festival of Creativity, The Geena Davis Institute on Gender in Media (Giaccardi et al. 2019 ) released data on gender representation in advertising between 2006 and 2016. Notably, women in ads were displayed in sexually revealing clothes and/or sexual posture six times more than men, that is, women were sexualized (Harker et al. 2005 ; Soley and Kurzbard 1986 ; Stankiewicz and Rosselli 2008 ). The underlying assumption governing the use of sexualized images in advertising is that these images entice consumers to purchase the associated products. In other words, the premise that has lived on for many years is that “sex sells.” However, advertising research has provided inconsistent results, with some studies showing that the use of sexualized ads leads to favorable responses by potential consumers (Grazer and Kessling 1995 ) and with other studies showing negative effects of ad sexualization (Bongiorno et al. 2013 ). Moreover, a recent meta-analysis by Wirtz et al. ( 2018 ) reported that men displayed more favorable attitudes toward sexualized women in ads than neutral ads whereas women showed the reverse pattern. In addition, no effects of sexualized female ads across gender were found on purchase intentions. Regarding ads with male model, the authors reported an overall negative effect of male models’ sexualization on attitudes and purchase intentions regardless of participants’ gender. Overall, given this inconsistent pattern of results, the “sex sells” assumption is only partially supported.

It is worth noting that the meta-analysis by Wirtz et al. ( 2018 ) includes a rather heterogenous set of studies, with different methodologies and operationalizations of sexualization, which may prevent researchers from gaining a clear understanding of the cognitive and behavioral processes triggered by sexualized advertisements. The goals of the present research are to overcome previous limitations and investigate mechanisms underlying attitudes and purchase intentions toward sexualized ads. To address these aims, we will (a) rely on a stringent methodology in which the same products are advertised by either sexualized or neutral ads (Studies 1–4) and (b) investigate an underlying mechanism by testing the mediating role of emotional responses toward the advertised products (Studies 3 and 4). In addition, we will explore the role of the socio-cultural milieu by testing the moderating role of gender role attitudes (Study 3), and we will extend previous research by distinguishing reactions of both women and men toward both female and male sexualized models in our ads (Study 4).

The goals of the present studies are particularly important because the basic assumption that “sex sells” appears to be taken for granted in advertising, which continues to use sexualized ads to promote products (e.g., Behm-Morawitz 2017 ; Verhellen et al. 2016 ) and disregards the negative psychological consequences that sexualized advertisements may have on viewers. Crucially, this representation negatively affects girls’ and women’s psychological as well as physical well-being (see Grabe et al. 2008 for a meta-analysis), and it also increases the endorsement of gender inequality norms, tolerance toward sexual harassment, and rape myth acceptance (see Ward 2016 , for a review). Hence, we argue that by investigating whether sexualized advertisements really sell, we address extremely relevant issues with regard to marketing, between gender relations (i.e., intergroup relations), intra-individual variables (e.g., gender role and sexist attitudes), and ethical and policymaking implications.

Sexualization in Advertising

According to objectification theory (Fredrickson and Roberts 1997 ), sexual objectification is a form of body reductionism that occurs whenever a person is fragmented into a collection of sexual body parts or functions, is considered as a silent decoration, and is evaluated solely on the basis of his or her appearance. Objectification theory has drawn from feminist studies (Bartky 1990 ; Nussbaum 1995 ) the idea that sexual objectification especially permeates women’s versus men’s lives because of the heterosexual and patriarchal nature of western societies. Objectification theory considers mass media, such as advertising, as one of the main contexts in which women’s sexual objectification is played out.

Content analyses, indeed, have shown that women are more likely than men to be depicted in sexualized ways in advertisements (e.g., Conley and Ramsey 2011 ; Paek et al. 2011 ; Stankiewicz and Rosselli 2008 ; Verhellen et al. 2016 ). Specifically in Italy, where the present research was conducted, researchers estimate that 81.27% of women in advertising are depicted as models, sexually available or pre-orgasmic, used as decoration (or “Greek fret”), or fragmented into body parts, whereas the sum of the corresponding categories for men is under 20% (19.95%, Italian Art Director Club report by Guastini et al. 2014 ). Interestingly, sexualization research has shown that these advertising images not only enact the objectification of women but also lead to negative consequences in the way women are viewed and in their physical and psychological well-being (see Loughnan and Pacilli 2014 ; Ward 2016 , for reviews).

Although sexualization is typically discussed in terms of women’s portrayals, researchers have shown that the male body is also increasingly sexualized and hypermuscularized in the media (Dafferner et al. 2019 ; Pope Jr. et al. 2001 ; Rohlinger 2002 ). Moreover, advertisements portraying male models as the muscular ideal increases men’s body dissatisfaction (Leit et al. 2002 ; Lorenzen et al. 2004 ). These worrisome findings both on women’s and men’s well-being pose the crucial question of whether the use of sexualization in advertisement is a commercially useful (as well as ethical) strategy to sell products. Therefore, we will investigate whether including a sexualized female or male model in an ad affects its efficacy.

The Efficacy of Sexualized Advertising

Previous research on attractiveness and purchase intentions toward products associated with sexualized female models has shown an inconclusive pattern of results. Some studies showed that the use of sexualized ads leads to increased positive attitudes toward them compared to neutral ads (Bello et al. 1983 ; Dudley 1999 ; Reichert et al. 2001 ) whereas other research found negative effects (Mittal and Lassar 2000 ; Peterson and Kerin 1977 ) especially by female viewers (Dahl et al. 2009 ; LaTour and Henthorne 1994 ; Sengupta and Dahl 2008 ). Similarly, inconsistent results across studies were found on purchase intentions (Bello et al. 1983 ; Dudley 1999 ; Grazer and Kessling 1995 ; LaTour and Henthorne 1994 ; Putrevu 2008 ).

Turning to the effects of sexualized male models, the research is very scarce and also leads to inconclusiveness. For example, Simpson et al. ( 1996 ) compared ads with different levels of male models’ sexualization (fully dressed, suggestive, nude) to a neutral (product only) condition. Findings suggested an opposite-gender effect, with women showing more favorable attitudes than men toward ads containing sexually suggestive male models. Using a similar design, Reidenbach and McCleary ( 1983 ) tested the effect of condition on purchase intentions, but found no effects of sexualization level. Moreover, Jones et al. ( 1998 ) compared ads with sexualized female and male models to neutral ads. Contrary to Simpson et al.’s ( 1996 ) findings, women’s attitudes did not differ across conditions and men reported higher positive attitudes toward female than male sexualized ads.

The literature on female and male sexualized ads poses some issues. First, the methodologies employed in these studies were not always rigorous. For example, Dudley ( 1999 ) compared ads with different levels of female model sexualization (swimsuit, topless, nude) to a neutral condition and found that the nude condition elicited more favorable attitudes, but no effects were found on purchase intentions. However, the four levels of ad sexualization were associated with different products, thus preventing clear conclusions on the unique effects of sexualization from being drawn (for similar issues see also, Belch et al. 1982 ; Ferguson et al. 2010 ; Grazer and Kessling 1995 ; Pan 2014 ; Putrevu 2008 ). Second, this literature is based on a large variety of methodologies, which complicates the interpretation of the unique role of female and male model sexualization in advertisements. For example, some studies used single-model ads (either female or male; Dudley 1999 ; Simpson et al. 1996 ), whereas other studies used ads portraying couples (Black and Morton 2017 ; LaTour and Henthorne 1994 ; Putrevu 2008 ).

In addition, although most studies were concerned with ad images, some research focused on the role of embedded sex-related text, thus making it difficult to understand whether the observed evidence was guided by the sexualized images per se (Aylesworth et al. 1999 ; Pan 2014 ). An additional element of variability is that some research was concerned with the effects of embedding a sexualized or/and violent ad in a media containing sex or/and violence (Bushman 2005 ; see Lull and Bushman 2015 , for a meta-analysis), an issue that goes beyond the goals of the present work. Overall, given that the methodology was not always rigorous and the materials used in this area of research were heterogeneous, it is difficult to determine the unique role of female and male model sexualization in advertisement.

Based on the large and heterogenous body of research we outlined, Wirtz and collaborators ( 2018 ) conducted an important meta-analysis on the effects of female and male sexualized ads. Concerning female model ads, the authors concluded that sexualized versus neutral ads lead to more favorable attitudes toward the ad by men, whereas women show less favorable attitudes toward sexualized versus neutral ads. In addition, the authors concluded that sexualized ads have no effects on purchase intentions compared to neutral ads. Regarding male model ads, Wirtz et al. ( 2018 ) reported an overall negative effect of male models’ sexualization on attitudes and purchase intentions regardless of participants’ gender. However, given the small number of studies available, they were unable to test whether a strong negative reaction from male participants drove these results (in line with Jones et al. 1998 ).

Overall, Wirtz et al. ( 2018 ) concluded that the heterogeneity of methodologies employed with female ads and the scarcity of research on male ads complicates the overall interpretation of the results, thus calling for future research. Moreover, further research is necessary because the research included in the present review is somewhat dated and may not capture what contemporary consumers do. In response to these calls, the goal of the present research is to test Wirtz et al.’s ( 2018 ) findings in the context of a rigorous methodology in which the same product is presented through a sexualized or neutral ad. To do so, we will conduct a series of studies including both female and male participants’ reactions toward ads with female or male models.

Emotional Reactions to Sexualized Advertisement

In the present work we also will study emotional responses by men and women toward sexualized versus neutral ads. This aim is backed by previous findings attesting that sexualized ads are viewed as having more emotional impact (i.e., are more powerful) than non-sexualized ads (Reichert et al. 2001 ). Importantly, there seem to be gender differences in the emotional appraisal of sexualized ads, although this evidence needs to be empirically addressed in a methodologically robust fashion. Previous research has shown that female nudity in advertisement triggers positive arousal for men, but negative arousal for women (LaTour 1990 ). In addition, Aylesworth et al. ( 1999 ) found that sexually suggestive subliminal messages embedded in ads increased both positive and negative feelings for men, but only negative feelings for women. An additional study by Reichert et al. ( 2007 ) found that both men and women reported more positive affective responses and attitudes toward ads picturing sexualized models of the other gender compared to same-gender models. Overall, although the measures and methodology are heterogeneous, these few studies suggest that men and women may differ in the emotional appraisal of sexualized ads, with men showing more favorable emotions and higher arousal toward female sexualized ads and women showing more negative emotions toward the same ads. At the same time women show more positive emotions toward male versus female sexualized ads.

Inspired by these prior findings, we reasoned that emotional appraisal may be a potential mechanism underlying product attitudes and purchase intentions. This reasoning is supported by research demonstrating that emotions affect persuasion processes (Dillard and Pfau 2002 ; Dillard and Wilson 1993 ) and by preliminary results indicating that emotions predict attitudes toward the ads (Aylesworth et al. 1999 ; Huang 2004 ), with positive emotions predicting more favorable, and negative emotions less favorable, attitudes toward the ads. Based on these preliminary results, we will measure positive and negative emotions appraised by women and men toward sexualized versus neutral ads and explore their mediating role in participants’ attitudes and purchase intentions toward the products (Studies 3 and 4).

The Role of Gender-Role Attitudes in Advertising

Researchers who have focused on whether sexualized ads actually sell products (Wirtz et al. 2018 , for a review) have mostly overlooked potential individual differences in terms of beliefs and attitudes that may modulate potential buyers’ appraisal of the ads (but see, for example, Wyllie et al. 2015 , for sexual self-schemas). In the context of stereotypical advertising research, some researchers have pointed to a possible role of traditional attitudes in gender relationships. For example, hostile sexism, as an overt hostility toward women that is composed of the drive forces of paternalism, gender differentiation, and heterosexuality (Glick and Fiske 1996 ), is positively related to favorable attitudes and purchase intentions toward stereotypically feminine ads (i.e., women as housewives; Zawisza et al. 2018 ). Moreover, attitudes toward gender roles have been studied in relation to stereotypical ads’ effectiveness (Zawisza and Cinnirella 2010 ), but to our knowledge such a relation has been overlooked in research regarding sexualization ads’ effectiveness.

Particularly relevant to the context of sexualized ads may be those attitudes in gender relationships that prescribe women to be submissive, sexy, and always available sexual partners (i.e., sexual objects), and men as dominant, sex-driven, and afraid of commitment (Ward 2002 ). We reasoned that these specific gender-role attitudes as well as hostile sexism may induce individuals to be more favorable toward sexualized advertising, which reflects the same traditional values. This prediction complements findings showing that chronic exposure to sexualized media increases endorsement of traditional gender role attitudes and hostile sexism (see Ward 2016 , for a review), suggesting a vicious cycle between sexualized advertising and societal values. Therefore, we deem it important to investigate how traditional gender role norms and attitudes modulate buyers’ reactions to sexualized advertisement.

The Present Research

In the present set of studies, we investigate attitudes and purchase intentions toward sexualized versus neutral ads (Studies 1–4). Specifically, in Study 1 we examine women’s and men’s responses to products advertised by either a sexualized female model or by a neutral ad. In line with Wirtz et al.’s ( 2018 ) meta-analysis, we hypothesize that men would show higher (Hypothesis 1a), and women lower (Hypothesis 1b), product attractiveness toward the sexualized than neutral ads. Given that Wirtz et al. ( 2018 ) found no effects on purchase intentions, we simply explore the effects of sexualized ads on purchase intentions.

In Study 2 we aim to replicate Study 1’s results and to also test whether gender role attitudes moderate the relation between ad sexualization and consumers’ reactions (i.e., product attractiveness, purchase intentions). We hypothesize that the more respondents, especially men, endorse a view of women as sexual objects and men as sex-driven, the higher the product attractiveness and purchase intentions in the sexualized (vs. control) ad condition (Hypothesis 2).

In Study 3 we aim at replicating Study 1’s and Study 2’s results and at identifying emotions as a possible mechanism underlying consumers’ reactions toward female model ads. Based on previous literature (Aylesworth et al. 1999 ; LaTour 1990 ), we hypothesize that women will show higher negative emotions in the sexualized than in the control condition (Hypothesis 3a), whereas no difference is predicted on positive emotions (Prediction 3b). Most important, we hypothesize that negative emotions evoked by exposure to sexualized ads represent a possible mediating mechanism underlying women’s decrement on product attractiveness and purchase intentions (Hypothesis 4). In Study 3 we will also test the moderating role of hostile sexism so that the higher hostile sexism toward women, especially by men, the higher the product attractiveness (Hypothesis 5a) and purchase intentions (Hypothesis 5b) in the sexualized than in the neutral ad condition.

Finally, in Study 4 we will extend the results of Studies 1–3 by distinguishing reactions of both women and men toward both female and male sexualized models in the ads. With regard to ads with male models, in line with Wirtz et al. ( 2018 ), we predict that both men and women will express lower product attractiveness and purchase intentions after viewing sexualized male ads than neutral ads (Hypothesis 6). Moreover, we hypothesize that participants’ emotional reactions will parallel participants responses on attractiveness and purchase intentions with lower positive emotions (Hypothesis 7a) and higher negative emotions (Hypothesis 7b) toward sexualized than neutral male ads. Concerning participants’ reactions toward sexualized ads with female models, we expect to replicate the prevalent pattern of results in Studies 1–3. Furthermore, we aim to replicate Study 3’s mediation results (see Hypothesis 4) and to explore an extension of the same mediation model to male model ads.

Study 1 focuses on female and male participants’ responses to female sexualized (versus neutral) ads in terms of product attractiveness and purchase intentions.

Participants and Design

We recruited 258 participants (153 women and 105 men). Three male and four female participants were eliminated because they did not give their consent to use their data after the debriefing. As such, the present analyses were conducted with 251 participants: 151 (60.2%) women and 100 (39.8%) men. Due to a procedure error the sample age is not available. Concerning participants’ education level, 12 (4.8%) participants received middle school diploma, 107 (42.6%) high school diploma, 82 (32.7%) Bachelor Degree, 47 (18.7%) Master Degree, and three (1.2%) PhD/Postgraduate Degree. Furthermore, the sample included 246 (98%) heterosexual participants, two lesbian women and one gay man, and one woman and one man who did not report their sexual orientation. The sensitivity power analyses (α = .05, Power 1 - β = .80, n  = 251) showed a minimal detectable effect (MDE) Cohen’s f  = .18. Therefore, the smallest effect size we were able to detect with the present sample size fell in the small effect area (Cohen 1988 ).

Procedure, Materials, and Measures

In addition to a snowball sampling procedure, the experimenters recruited participants via Facebook and Instagram posts and via messages to acquaintances. The experiment was described as investigating people’s attitudes. Those who were interested in participating in the study were asked to access a link to the online questionnaire. The experiment was conducted via the SurveyMonkey platform, and participants volunteered to participate without monetary compensation. After accessing the link and providing informed consent, participants were randomly assigned to view either five sexualized or five neutral ads depending on the experimental condition. Respondents were instructed to focus their attention on each advertisement because they would be later asked to indicate their attitudes toward them. After the manipulation, participants completed a series of measures in the same order as they are presented in the following. At the end, participants provided their socio-demographic information, read a written debriefing, and also received the opportunity to have an additional oral debriefing. Lastly, they were asked to give final consent to use their data in an anonymous and aggregated form. The ethical committee of the University of Padova (Italy) approved the present experimental protocol including all four studies. All studies were conducted in Italian.

Participants were exposed to either five sexualized or five neutral ads. The advertised products were: a kitchen, beer, a mattress, mozzarella, and eyeglasses. The sexualized ads were selected from the internet. Each of them depicted a woman in a highly sexual suggestive manner (i.e., in a provocative pose and revealing a substantial extent of nudity, which are the characteristics commonly used in the literature to define sexualization; Hatton and Trautner 2011 ; Pacilli et al. 2016 ). In all studies, the ad models were White and fit the thin body ideal. Only the kitchen ad portrayed a couple in a pose suggestive of sexual activity, a material inconsistency that will be addressed in Study 2. To create the neutral ad material, Photoshop was used to modify the sexualized ads by deleting the sexualized woman. Therefore, each neutral ad portrayed the same product of the same brand as the corresponding sexualized ad with no woman included. Both sexualized and neutral ads included the original text of the real ads.

Product Attractiveness

Participants were asked to indicate the extent to which they were attracted to each product on five items rated from 1 ( Not at all ) to 5 ( Very much ). The items were created ad-hoc for each product and therefore different from each other, an inconsistency that we addressed in Study 2. For example, the questions for the mozzarella ad were: “Does this image make you want to eat mozzarella?,” “How tasty do you think this mozzarella is?,” “How fresh do you think this mozzarella is?,” “How real do you think this mozzarella is?,” “Does this image make you want to try this mozzarella?” In all studies, we calculated an overall product attractiveness index by averaging responses across the items for each product and then across the products (α = .93).

Purchase Intentions

Next, participants were asked to indicate their purchase intentions for each product by responding on a scale ranging from 1 ( Not at all ) to 5 ( Very much ) to the following items: “Would you buy this product?,” “Would you recommend this product to others?,” “Would you like to buy this product?” The scale was created ad hoc as an adaptation from previous measures (Reichert et al. 2007 ). In all studies, we calculated an overall index of purchase intentions by averaging responses first across the items for each product and then across the products (α = .90).

In addition, other measures were taken (i.e., Participants’ habits and familiarity with the products, Likelihood to Sexually Harass and Dehumanization of women; see the Online Supplement for more information).

The product attractiveness index was M  = 2.54 ( SD  = .68); the purchase intentions average was M  = 2.27 ( SD  = .69). To test Hypothesis 1a and Hypothesis 1b, we conducted a MANOVA on product attractiveness and purchase intentions with condition (sexualized vs. control) and participant gender (men vs. women) as between-subjects factors. The multivariate main effects of condition, Pillai’s trace = .13, F (2, 246) = 18.67, p <  .001, η p 2  = .13, and gender, Pillai’s trace = .03, F (2, 246) = 4.13, p =  .017, η p 2  = .03, as well as the Condition x Gender interaction, Pillai’s trace = .06, F (2, 246) = 8.09, p <  .001, η p 2  = .06, were significant.

Concerning univariate effects on product attractiveness, main effects of gender, F (1, 247) = 7.76, p  = .006, η p 2  = .03, and condition, F (1, 247) = 28.88, p  < .001, η p 2  = .10, were found, which were qualified by a significant interaction between condition and participants’ gender, F (1, 247) = 15.64, p  < .001, η p 2  = .05. Post-hoc comparisons with Bonferroni correction revealed that, in line with Hypothesis 1b, women reported lower product attractiveness in the sexualized ( M  = 2.18, SD  = .62) than in the control condition ( M  = 2.94, SD  = .47; p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = 1.29). In addition, but contrary to Hypothesis 1a, men showed no difference between the sexualized ( M  = 2.73, SD  = .66) and the control condition ( M  = 2.84, SD  = .55; p  = .329, Cohen’s d  = .18). Moreover, men in the sexualized condition showed higher product attractiveness compared to women in the same condition ( p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = .87). No statistically significant difference was found between men and women in the control condition ( p  = .459, Cohen’s d  = .21).

Univariate effects on purchase intentions also showed significant main effects of gender, F (1, 247) = 7.07, p  = .006, η p 2  = .03, and condition, F (1, 247) = 4.15, p  = .002, η p 2  = .03, as well as a significant two-way interaction between condition and gender, F (1, 247) = 9.19, p  = .003, η p 2  = .03. Post-hoc comparisons with Bonferroni correction revealed that women reported lower purchase intentions in the sexualized ( M  = 1.98, SD  = .64) than control condition ( M  = 2.53, SD  = .59; p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = .88), whereas men showed no difference across conditions ( M sexualized  = 2.49, SD  = .67; M control  = 2.50, SD  = .65; p  = .919). Moreover, the gender difference was statistically significant in the sexualized condition ( p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = .77) but non-significant in the control condition, ( p  = .872).

Study 1 showed a series of important results. First, in line with predictions (Hypothesis 1b) and previous research (Wirtz et al. 2018 ), women showed significantly lower attractiveness toward products advertised by sexualized female models compared to neutral ads. Second, contrary to predictions from previous research (Wirtz et al. 2018 ; Hypothesis 1a), men did not show higher product attractiveness after viewing female sexualized than neutral ads. The same pattern of results was found on purchase intentions, thus contradicting Wirtz et al.’ ( 2018 ) conclusions regarding the lack of effects on purchase intentions. However, one limitation of Study 1 was that the ad materials were not pretested, an issue overcome in Study 2.

The aim of Study 2 is to replicate Study 1’s results and to also test the moderating role of gender role attitudes related to the notion of women as sexual objects and men as sex-driven (see Hypothesis 2).

Participants

Two-hundred Italian participants (108 women, and 92 men) recruited online volunteered to participate in the present study. Two male participants were eliminated because one was younger than 18 years and one older than 60 years of age, and one female participant was eliminated because she did not sign the final consent. The final sample included 197 participants, 107 women (54%) and 90 men (46%), with an age range of 18–55 ( M  = 28.47, SD  = 11.62). Twenty-two (11%) participants received middle school diploma, 139 (71%) high school diploma, 12 (7%) Bachelor Degree, 18 (9%) Master Degree, and 5 (2%) Ph.D/Postgraduate Degree. Most participants ( n  = 187, 95%) identified as heterosexual, eight (4%) identified as gay men, lesbian women or bisexual, and two participants (1%) refrained from answering. Note that excluding non-heterosexual participants did not change results. The sensitivity power analyses (α = .05, Power 1 - β = .80, n  = 197) showed a minimal detectable effect (MDE) Cohen’s f  = .20. Therefore, the smallest effect size we were able to detect with the present sample size fell in the small effect area (Cohen 1988 ).

The procedure was the same as in Study 1. Unlike Study 1, at the end participants reported what they thought the aim of the study was. Finally, participants filled out the moderator measure (i.e., Attitudes that women are sexual objects and men are sex-driven).

Participants were exposed to six ads depicting a sexualized woman or six neutral ads depicting only the same product. The products were: male shoes, vodka, mattress, beer, cereals, and male cologne. Importantly, all ads were real and carefully pretested (see the Online Supplement which includes an example of an ad).

Unlike Study 1, for each ad, two items measured product attractiveness (i.e., ‘How much does this product attract you?’; ‘How attractive is this product?’), on a scale ranging from 1 ( Not at all ) to 7 ( Very much ) (overall α = .91).

Purchase intention was measured as in Study 1 except for the addition of the item “Does this image make you want to try this product?” on a scale ranging from 1 ( Not at all ) to 7 ( Very much ) (overall α = .94).

Attitudes that Women are Sexual Objects and Men are Sex-Driven

We measured participants’ endorsement of socially shared cultural attitudes that women are sexual objects and men are sex-driven via two subscales of the revised version of the Attitudes about Dating and Sexual Relationships scale (Ward 2002 ). Confirmatory factor analyses supported the structural validity of the scale (Ward 2002 ). The women are sexual objects subscale measures the extent to which women’s role is to be sexual objects (8 items; e.g., “Women should spend a lot of time trying to be pretty”; α = .83), the men are sex-driven subscale measures the belief that men are driven by sex needs (7 items; e.g., “It’s difficult for men to resist sexual urges and to remain monogamous”; α = .79). Scales ranged from 1 ( Not at all ) to 7 ( Very much ).

Notice that other measures were also taken (i.e., Participants’ habits and familiarity with the products, Dehumanization of women, and Enjoyment of sexualization; see the Online Supplement ).

Product Attractiveness and Purchase Intentions

The product attractiveness index was M  = 2.90 ( SD  = 1.23); the purchase intentions overall mean was 2.50 ( SD  = 1.07). To test a replication of Study 1’s results, we conducted a MANOVA on product attractiveness and purchase intentions with condition (sexualized vs. control) and participant gender (men vs. women) as between-subjects factors. The multivariate main effects of condition, Pillai’s trace = .05, F (2, 192) = 4.75, p =  .010, η p 2  = .05, and gender, Pillai’s trace = .17, F (2, 192) = 19.05, p <  .001, η p 2  = .17, as well as the Condition x Gender interaction, Pillai’s trace = .15, F (2, 192) = 16.54, p <  .001, η p 2  = .15, were significant.

Concerning the univariate effects on product attractiveness, a main effect of gender was found, F (1, 193) = 33.75, p  < .001, η p 2  = .15. Most important, the Condition x Gender interaction was significant F (1, 193) = 30.02, p  < .001, η p 2  = .13. Different from Study 1 and in line with Hypothesis 1a, post-hoc analyses (Bonferroni adjusted) showed that men indicated higher product attractiveness in the sexualized ( M  = 3.85, SD  = 1.44) than in the control condition ( M  = 2.96, SD  = .98; p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = .72). In line with Study 1 and Hypothesis 1b, women indicated lower product attractiveness after viewing sexualized ( M  = 2.12, SD  = .97) than neutral ads ( M  = 2.91, SD  = .80; p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = .88). Moreover, men in the sexualized condition indicated higher product attractiveness compared to women in the same condition ( p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = 1.45). No statistically significant difference was found comparing male and female participants in the control condition ( p  = .821).

Concerning the univariate effects on purchase intentions, a significant main effect of gender, F (1,193) = 15.95, p  < .001, η p 2  = .08, was qualified by a significant interaction with condition, F (1,193) = 15.00, p  < .001, η p 2  = .07. Men did not show different purchase intentions after exposure to sexualized ( M  = 3.02, SD  = 1.34) than neutral ads ( M  = 2.65, SD  = .84; p  = .073, Cohen’s d  = .33). In contrast, women showed lower purchase intentions in the sexualized ( M  = 1.90, SD  = .87) than in the control condition ( M  = 2.63, SD  = .83; p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = .86). In addition, in the sexualized condition men showed significantly higher purchase intentions than women ( p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = 1.02), whereas the same comparison was not significant after exposure to neutral ads ( p  = .934).

Moderation Analyses

The overall mean on the “women are sexual objects” index was M  = 3.30 ( SD  = 1.03); the “men are sex driven” average was M  = 3.57 ( SD  = 1.22). To test Hypothesis 2, using PROCESS (Model n.3; Hayes 2013 ) we entered condition (sexualized = 1, control = 0) as the independent variable, gender (women = 1, men = 0) as the first moderator and either “women are sexual objects” or “men are sex-driven” as the second moderator (continuous, centered). The Condition x Gender interactions were significant both on product attractiveness and purchase intentions, thus mimicking ANOVA effects ( ts  > 3.28, ps  < .002). However, contrary to Hypothesis 2, the three-way interactions among predictors were not significant both on product attractiveness and purchase intentions ( ts  < .80, ps  > .425). Thus, the moderating role of gender attitudes related to the notion that “women are sexual objects” and “men are sex-driven” was not supported in the current study.

In line with Hypothesis 1b and results of Study 1, Study 2 demonstrated that women showed significantly lower product attractiveness after exposure to sexualized female ads than neutral ads. In addition, in line with Hypothesis 1a, the opposite pattern was observed for men who showed higher product attractiveness toward sexualized female ads than neutral ads. This result contradicts results of Study 1, in which men were unaffected by condition. The discrepancy between Study 1 and Study 2 calls for more research on product attractiveness, which is one of the goals of Study 3.

Regarding purchase intentions, Study 2 fully replicated Study 1’s results, showing that women had lower intentions to purchase products in the sexualized than in the control condition. This overall result is important because it moves forward the research in this area by showing that women respond negatively to female sexualization. As important is the result that men’s purchase intentions were not affected by ad condition, in line with Study 1. In addition, contrary to our hypothesis (Hypothesis 2), attitudes that women are sexual objects and men are sex-driven did not moderate the pattern of results, leaving the question of the role of sociocultural attitudes unanswered, an issue that will be further investigated in Study 3.

The first goal of Study 3 is to further investigate the effects of female model ad sexualization on product attractiveness and purchase intentions. Specifically, we will investigate emotions as a possible underlying mechanism of women’s decreased responses toward sexualized female ads (see Hypothesis 3a, Prediction 3b, Hypothesis 4). Given that men’s product attractiveness and purchase intentions did not consistently vary depending on ad sexualization, no effects on their emotions were hypothesized.

A second goal of Study 3 is to further explore the role of the sociocultural milieu by testing hostile sexist attitudes toward women as a potential moderator of the relation between sexualization in advertising and consumers’ responses (see Hypothesis 5a and Hypothesis 5b). Although in Study 2 the gender attitudes that women are sexual objects and men are sex-driven produced no effects, we predicted that hostile sexism may function as a moderator. Indeed, although the two constructs may be related to each other, we reasoned that hostile sexism represents a construct different from gender attitudes (Chen et al. 2009 ; Glick and Fiske 1996 ).

In addition, previous research has shown that consumers’ purchase intentions may be positively affected by the congruency between the gender-relevance of the product and the level of sexualization of the ad (e.g., Black and Morton 2017 ; Simpson et al. 1996 ; see also Wirtz et al. 2018 for a meta-analysis). Gender-relevant products are those products that are congruent with gender stereotypes—for example, masculine-typed liquor (Grazer and Kessling 1995 ), and feminine-typed fragrances (LaTour 1990 ; Reichert et al. 2001 ). Therefore, for a more complete methodology we included both gender-relevant and gender-irrelevant products and explored whether gender relevance would modulate the results.

Two-hundred and two participants (105 women, 97 men) recruited through advertisement in social networks voluntarily participated in the present study. Three male and one female participant were excluded because they did not sign the final consent, therefore the final sample included 198 participants: 104 (52.5%) women and 94 (47.5%) men. The sensitivity power analyses (α = .05, Power 1 - β = .80, n  = 198) showed a minimal detectable effect (MDE) Cohen’s f  = .20, which fell in the small effect area (Cohen 1988 ). Participants’ age ranged from 18 to 67 years-old ( M  = 32.11, SD  = 12.11). Seventeen (8.6%) participants received middle school diploma, 101 (51.0%) high school diploma, 45 (22.7%) Bachelor Degree, 32 (16.2%) Master Degree and three (1.5%) Ph.D/Postgraduate Degree. The sample was mostly composed of heterosexual men and women ( n  = 175, 88.4%). Thirteen men and women (6.6%) declared to be gay or lesbian (7 women, 6 men), three bisexuals (1 woman, 2 men), one woman declared herself “queer” (2%), and five participants did not respond (3%). Please note that results did not change when non-heterosexual respondents were excluded from analyses.

The procedure was similar to Study 2. Unlike Study 2, the study was a 2 (participant gender) × 2 (condition: sexualized vs. neutral ads, between-subject variable) × 2 (product’s gender relevance: gender-relevant vs. gender-irrelevant product, within-subject variable) mixed design. As in Study 2 participants were randomly exposed to either six female sexualized ads or six neutral ads; however, among the six ads three included gender-relevant products (i.e., vodka, perfume, and beer) and three gender-irrelevant products (i.e., chewing gum, sneakers, and toilet paper), a classification based on previous literature (Grazer and Kessling 1995 ). Thus, new ads were pretested together with the ads used in Study 2 (see the Online Supplement ). The presentation order of the gender-ir/relevant ads was randomized. After viewing each ad participants rated its product attractiveness and indicated their purchase intentions. To make the manipulation salient again all ads were presented again in a random order and participants rated their emotions. At the end, as in Study 2, participants filled out the moderator questionnaire (i.e., hostile sexism together with a filler scale on environmentalism).

Product attractiveness and purchase intentions were measured as in Study 2. Given that participants were presented with three gender-relevant products and three gender-irrelevant products, we calculated three indexes of product attractiveness (gender-relevant product attractiveness: α = .85; gender-irrelevant product attractiveness: α = .79; product attractiveness across all six ads: α = .89) and three of purchase intentions (gender-relevant purchase intentions: α = .92; gender-irrelevant purchase intentions: α = .90; purchase intentions across all six ads: α = .94). Note that participants’ habits and familiarity with the products were also measured but did not affect the results (see the Online Supplement ).

Participants were asked to indicate the extent to which they had experienced some specific emotions after viewing the ads on a scale from 1 ( Not at all ) to 7 ( Very much ). In line with previous studies measuring emotions (Albarello and Rubini 2012 ; Vaes et al. 2003 ), we measured eight positive emotions and nine negative emotions in mixed order (i.e., positive emotions: attraction, admiration, excitement, joy, pleasure, contentment, passion, and surprise, α = .91; negative emotions: annoyance, anger, rage, contempt, disappointment, disgust, fear, sadness, and agitation, α = .90).

Hostile Sexism

Participants completed the 11-item Hostile Sexism (HS) subscale of the Ambivalent Sexism Inventory (ASI, Glick and Fiske 1996 ), validated in Italian by Manganelli Rattazzi et al. ( 2008 ). Structural validity of the Italian version of ASI was supported by both exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses and reported internal consistency for Hostile Sexism was α = .87 (Manganelli Rattazzi et al. 2008 ). Participants provided their responses to the items (e.g., “Women seek to gain power by getting control over men”; α = .91) on a scale from 1 ( Not at all likely ) to 7 ( Very likely ). Scores were averaged across items such that higher scores indicate stronger endorsement of hostile sexism.

Product attractiveness and purchase intentions

The overall mean on the product attractiveness index was M  = 2.81 ( SD  = 1.16); the purchase intentions average was M  = 2.59 ( SD  = 1.07). As in Study 1 and Study 2, we conducted a MANOVA on product attractiveness and purchase intentions with condition (sexualized vs. control) and participants’ gender (men vs. women) as between-subjects factors. The multivariate main effects of condition, Pillai’s trace = .13, F (2, 193) = 14.89, p <  .001, η p 2  = .13, and gender, Pillai’s trace = .05, F (2, 193) = 5.22, p =  .006, η p 2  = .05, as well as the Condition x Gender interaction, Pillai’s trace = .03, F (2, 193) = 3.41, p =  .035, η p 2  = .03 were significant.

Concerning the univariate effects on product attractiveness, the main effect of gender, F (1, 194) = 8.42, p  = .004, η p 2  = .04 (see Table 1 for descriptive statistics), was qualified by a significant Condition x Gender interaction F (1, 194) = 5.93, p  = .016, η p 2  = .03. In line with Study 1, Study 2, and Hypothesis 1b, as shown in Table 1 , women reported lower product attractiveness after exposure to sexualized than neutral ads ( p  = .002, Cohen’s d  = .70). In line with Study 1 and contrary to Hypothesis 1a, men did not show different product attractiveness across conditions ( p  = .727). Moreover, men in the sexualized condition indicated higher product attractiveness compared to women in the same condition ( p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = .68) whereas no gender difference was found in the control condition ( p  = .743).

Concerning the univariate effects on purchase intentions significant main effects of gender, F (1,194) = 4.71, p  = .031, η p 2  = .02, and condition, F (1,194) = 13.51, p  < .001, η p 2  = .06, were found (see Table 1 for descriptive statistics). Importantly, the interaction between condition and gender was significant, F (1,194) = 6.85, p  = .010, η p 2  = .03. In line with Study 1 and Study 2, as shown in Table 1 , men did not show different purchase intentions after exposure to sexualized or neutral ads ( p  = .466). In contrast, women showed lower purchase intentions in the sexualized than in the control condition ( p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = 1.04). Moreover, in the sexualized condition men showed significantly higher purchase intentions than women ( p  = .001, Cohen’s d  = .64), whereas the same comparison was not significant after exposure to neutral ads ( p  = .753).

To explore the effects of product gender-relevance, we conducted repeated measure ANOVAs separately on product attractiveness and purchase intentions including condition and gender as between subjects factors and gender-relevance of the product (gender-relevant, gender-irrelevant) as the within-subjects variable. Neither the main effects of gender-relevance, Fs (1, 194) < 2.55, ps  > .112, η p 2 s < .01, nor the three-way interactions with condition and gender, Fs (1, 194) < .35, ps  > .552, η p 2 s < .002, were statistically significant, whereas the other effects remained statistically significant (see the Online Supplement for additional results).

The average negative emotions score was M  = 2.19 ( SD  = 1.27); the positive emotions’ average was M  = 2.08 ( SD  = 1.14). To test Hypothesis 3a and Prediction 3b, we conducted a MANOVA on negative and positive emotions with condition (sexualized vs. control) and participant gender (men vs. women) as between factors. The multivariate effects of condition, Pillai’s trace = .22, F (2, 193) = 27.60, p <  .001, η p 2  = .22, gender, Pillai’s trace = .06, F (2, 193) = 6.39, p =  .002, η p 2  = .06, and Condition x Gender, Pillai’s trace = .08, F (2, 193) = 8.80, p <  .001, η p 2  = .08 were significant.

Concerning univariate effects, a main effect of gender emerged on both negative emotions, F (2,194) = 8.70, p =  .004, η p 2  = .04, and positive emotions, F (2,194) = 7.47, p =  .007, η p 2  = .04, and a significant main effect of condition was found only on negative emotions, F (1,194) = 51.46, p  < .001, η p 2  = .19 (see Table 1 for descriptive statistics). Importantly, as predicted, both negative, F (1, 194) = 15.17, p  < .001, η p 2  = .06, and positive, F (1, 194) = 6.49, p  = .012, η p 2  = .03, emotions experienced by participants were affected by the interaction between condition and participants’ gender. Specifically, in line with Hypothesis 3a (see Table 1 ), women reported significantly more negative emotions after exposure to sexualized than neutral ads ( p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = 1.50), and a similar pattern was observed for men ( p  = .025, Cohen’s d  = .50). Moreover, in the sexualized condition women showed significantly more negative emotions than men, ( p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = .76) whereas the comparison was not statistically significant for the control condition ( p  = .506). With reference to positive emotions and in line with Prediction 3b (see Table 1 ), both women’s and men’s levels of positive emotions did not differ across conditions ( p s > .072). The only significant comparison is that women exposed to sexualized ads manifested lower positive emotions than men ( p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = .69), whereas this difference was not significant in the control condition ( p  = .896). Please notice that the correlation between positive and negative emotions was r (97) = −.24, p  = .018 for men, and r (105) = −.29, p  = .003 for women.

Mediation by Emotions

To test Hypothesis 4, we computed an overall index of emotional negativity by subtracting responses on positive emotions from those on negative emotions, thus the higher the index the higher the level of negative emotions reported by participants. A moderated mediation analysis was performed through PROCESS (Model n° 8, Hayes 2013 ) on product attractiveness. The model included condition (control = 0, sexualized = 1) as the independent variable, emotional negativity (continuous, centered) as the mediator, and participants’ gender (men = 0, women = 1) as the moderator assessing its effects both on the mediator and on the dependent variable. The overall model was significant ( R 2  = .51), F (4, 193) = 49.91, p  < .001. Importantly, in line with Hypothesis 4, a significant indirect negative effect of condition through emotional negativity emerged specifically for women ( b  = −.91, SE  = .15, 95% CI [−1.21, −.63]) (with 5000 bootstrap samples). Specifically, sexualized (vs. neutral) ads increased women’s negative emotions, which in turn decreased their product attractiveness scores. This was not the case for men ( b  = −.04, SE  = .16, 95% CI [−.35, .29]).

We conducted the same analysis on purchase intentions. The overall model was significant ( R 2  = .47), F (4, 193) = 42.23, p  < .001. Supporting Hypothesis 4, the indirect effect of condition through emotional negativity was significant specifically for women ( b  = −.77, SE  = .14, 95% CI [−1.06, −.51]) (with 5000 bootstrap samples). In other words, similar to product attractiveness, women reported lower purchase intentions after viewing sexualized ads than neutral ads because of their higher level of negative emotions. Again, this was not the case for men ( b  = −.03, SE  = .14, 95% CI [−.31, .24]).

Moderation by Hostile Sexism

The overall mean on the hostile sexism index was M  = 3.36 ( SD  = 1.21). Using PROCESS (Model n.3; Hayes 2013 ), we tested the moderating role of hostile sexism on both product attractiveness (Hypothesis 5a) and purchase intentions (Hypothesis 5b). Specifically, we entered condition (sexualized = 1, control = 0) as the independent variable, participants’ gender (women = 1, men = 0) as the first moderator and hostile sexism as the second moderator (continuous, centered). Concerning product attractiveness, although the overall model was statistically significant, F (7,190) = 5.26, p  < .001, the model including the three-way interaction of Condition x Gender x Hostile sexism ( b  = −.48, t  = −1.84, p  = .067, 95% CI [−1.01, .03]) did not increase the amount of variance explained (Δ R 2  = .01, R 2  = .16, p  = .067), thus not supporting Hypothesis 5a regarding the moderating role of hostile sexism on product attractiveness.

With respect to purchase intentions, analysis revealed a significant two-way interaction between condition and hostile sexism ( b  = .36, t  = 2.02, p  = .044, 95% CI [.01, .71]) qualified by a significant three-way interaction among Condition x Gender x Hostile sexism ( b  = −.53, t  = −2.26, p  = .025, 95% CI [−1.021, −.07]), which significantly increased the amount of variance explained (Δ R 2  = .02, p  = .025; overall model: R 2  = .21, F (7, 190) = 7.13, p  < .001). Supporting Hypothesis 5b, the higher men’s hostile sexism in the sexualized condition, the higher their purchase intentions ( b  = .45, SE =  .13, t  = 3.42, p  = .001, 95% CI [.19, .70]). In contrast, hostile sexism was unrelated to purchase intentions for men in the control condition ( b  = .08, SE =  .12, t  = .68, p  = .497, 95% CI [−.16, .33]) as well as for women in the sexualized condition ( b  = .13, SE =  .10, t  = 1.29, p  = .198, 95% CI [−.07, .34]). Unexpectedly, the higher the hostile sexism the higher the purchase intentions also for women in the control condition ( b  = .31, SE =  .12, t  = 2.66, p  = .008, 95% CI [08, .53]).

In Study 3 women showed lower product attractiveness after exposure to sexualized female ads than neutral ads confirming Hypothesis 1b, whereas, contrary to Hypothesis 1a, men were unaffected by ads’ sexualization. This pattern of results replicated Study 1’s results as a whole and Study 2’s results for women. Interestingly, the same pattern was observed on purchase intentions, which fully replicated both Study 1’s and Study 2’s results. Moreover, contrary to Wirtz et al. ( 2018 ), the gender relevance of the product produced no effects. Therefore, this variable was not measured in the following study.

In addition, a series of important results were found on emotions. First, in line with Hypothesis 3a, women showed higher negative emotions after exposure to sexualized than neutral ads. Moreover, in line with Prediction 3b, women’s positive emotions did not differ across conditions. Second, the same pattern was found on men’s emotions. Most important, in line with Hypothesis 4, emotional negativity was shown to be the mechanism specifically underlying women’s decrement in product attractiveness and purchase intentions toward sexualized female models (vs. neutral) ads. Finally, consistent with Hypothesis 5b, an interesting result was that the higher the level of hostile sexism by men, the higher their purchase intentions after viewing sexualized than neutral ads.

In Study 4 we sought to replicate the overall pattern of results in Studies 1–3 regarding female model ads. In addition, responding to Wirtz et al.’s ( 2018 ) call, we aim at investigating the effects of sexualization of male model ads on both men’s and women’s emotional reactions, product attractiveness, and purchase intentions (see Hypothesis 6, Hypotheses 7a and 7b).

Fully 212 participants ( n  = 114 women, n  = 98 men), contacted through social media, voluntarily completed the online questionnaire. Because one female and four male participants were excluded because they did not sign the final consent, the final sample included 207 participants: 113 (54.6%) women and 94 (45.4%) men. The sensitivity power analyses (α = .05, Power 1 - β = .80) on the available sample showed a minimal detectable effect (MDE) Cohen’s f  = .19, which falls in the small effect area (Cohen 1988 ). Participants’ age ranged from 19 to 63 years-old ( M  = 29.99, SD  = 10.65). The sample education level was: one participant (.5%) elementary school diploma, 10 (4.8%) middle school diploma, 107 (51.7%) high school diploma, 32 (15.5%) Bachelor Degree, 51 (24.6%) Master Degree and 6 (2.9%) Ph.D/Postgraduate Degree. Furthermore, the sample was mostly heterosexual ( n  = 184, 88.9%), 10 (4.8%) participants identified as gay, lesbian, or bisexual, six (2.9%) declared other sexual identities, and seven (3.4%) did not respond.

The procedure was similar to Study 3. The major difference was that the experimental design was 2 (participants’ gender) × 2 (condition: sexualized vs. control) × 2 (targets’ gender: male vs. female model) with the last variable within-subjects. Specifically, among the six presented ads, three (i.e., vodka, eyeglasses, and cologne) included male models and three (i.e., chewing gum, beer, and sneakers) included female models. Note that male and female model ads were pretested (see the Online Supplement ) and presented in randomized order.

The same scales as in Studies 2 and 3 were used to measure product attractiveness and purchase intentions. We calculated product attractiveness indexes by averaging responses to the two attractiveness items separately for male model (α = .80) and female model (α = .84) ads. The same procedure was followed for purchase intentions (male model ads: α = .90; female model ads: α = .92).

The same emotions as in Study 3 were used in the present study. The only difference was that the manipulation re-activation was done separately for male and female model ads. Participants were first presented with the manipulation re-activation of only the male ads and completed the emotions scale referring to the male ads, and then they were presented with the manipulation re-activation of the female ads and responded to the emotion scale for female ads. The presentation order was counterbalanced and produced no effect, Fs (1, 204) < 2.94, ps  > .091. A good reliability was found both for positive (female ads: α = .92; male ads: α = .90) and negative (female ads: α = .89; male ads: α = .89) emotions.

Please notice that other measures were assessed (i.e., Participants’ habits and familiarity with the products; Acceptance of the use of female and male body to sell products; Inclusion of objectified women/men in the overall gender category; see the Online Supplement ).

The overall mean on the product attractiveness index for female model ads was M  = 3.01 ( SD  = 1.31), whereas for male model ads was M  = 2.44 ( SD  = 1.20). The purchase intentions average for female model ads was M  = 2.88 ( SD  = 1.29), whereas for male model ads was M  = 2.33 ( SD  = 1.15). Following previous studies’ statistical strategy, we conducted a MANOVA on male and female model ads’ product attractiveness and purchase intentions indices with condition (sexualized vs. control) and participants’ gender (men vs. women) as between-subjects factors. The multivariate main effects of condition, Pillai’s trace = .18, F (4, 200) = 10.82, p <  .001, η p 2  = .18, and gender, Pillai’s trace = .14, F (4, 200) = 7.95, p <  .001, η p 2  = .14, as well as the Condition x Gender interaction, Pillai’s trace = .10, F (4, 200) = 5.67, p <  .001, η p 2  = .10, were significant.

Concerning product attractiveness toward female model ads, the univariate analyses revealed that the significant main effects of condition, F (1, 203) = 15.29, p  < .001, η p 2  = .06, and gender, F (1, 203) = 13.80, p  < .001, η p 2  = .05, were qualified by a significant Condition x Gender interaction, F (1, 203) = 15.96, p  < .001, η p 2  = .06. As shown in Table 2 , supporting previous studies and contrary to Hypothesis 1a, men did not show any significant difference between product attractiveness toward female sexualized versus neutral ads ( p  = .955), whereas, in line with Hypothesis 1b, women reported lower product attractiveness after exposure to female sexualized than neutral ads ( p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = 1.12). In addition, female sexualized ads elicited higher product attractiveness by men than women ( p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = 1.04). Men’s and women’s product attractiveness did not differ in the control condition ( p  = .840).

Regarding univariate analyses on product attractiveness toward male model ads, the interaction between condition and gender was not significant, F (1, 203) = .57, p  = .451, η p 2  = .002. The significant main effects of gender, F (1, 203) = 5.10, p  = .025, η p 2  = .02, and condition, F (1, 203) = 36.01, p  < .001, η p 2  = .15, showed that women were more attracted than men toward products advertised by male models, but that regardless their gender, participants showed lower product attractiveness toward sexualized male ads than neutral ads, in line with Hypothesis 6 (see Table 2 ).

As for univariate analyses on purchase intentions toward female ads, the significant main effects of condition, F (1, 203) = 17.64, p  < .001, η p 2  = .07, and gender, F (1, 203) = 9.03, p  = .003, η p 2  = .04, were qualified by a significant Condition x Gender interaction, F (1, 203) = 12.67, p  < .001, η p 2  = .05. As shown in Table 2 , in line with Studies 1–3 women reported lower purchase intentions after exposure to female sexualized than neutral ads ( p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = 1.06) whereas men were unaffected by condition ( p  = .665). Moreover, men showed significantly higher purchase intentions than female participants in the sexualized condition ( p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = .95). Men’s and women’s product attractiveness did not differ in the control condition ( p  = .690).

Finally, concerning purchase intentions toward male model ads, similarly to product attractiveness, univariate analyses showed that the interaction between condition and gender was not significant, F (1, 203) = .30, p  = .582, η p 2  = .001. The main effect of condition was the only significant effect, F (1, 203) = 26.33, p  < .001, η p 2  = .11. In line with Hypothesis 6, regardless of their gender, participants showed lower purchase intentions toward sexualized male ads than neutral ads (see Table 2 ).

The average negative emotions score for female model ads was M  = 1.78 ( SD  = 1.05), whereas for male model ads was M  = 1.90 ( SD  = 1.10). The positive emotions’ average for female model was M  = 2.11 ( SD  = 1.18), whereas for male model ads was M  = 1.96 ( SD  = 1.07). To test our predictions, we conducted a MANOVA on positive and negative emotions toward male and female models’ ads with condition (sexualized vs. control) and participants’ gender (men vs. women) as between-subjects factors. The multivariate main effects of condition, Pillai trace = .12, F (4, 200) = 7.07, p <  .001, η p 2  = .12, and gender, Pillai trace = .09, F (4, 200) = 5.27, p <  .001, η p 2  = .09, as well as the Condition x Gender interaction Pillai trace = .09, F (4, 200) = 4.77, p =  .011, η p 2  = .09, were significant.

With reference to emotional reactions toward female model ads, univariate analyses showed a main effect of gender on both negative ( F (1, 203) = 4.79, p =  .030, η p 2  = .02) and positive emotions ( F (1, 203) = 6.60, p =  .011, η p 2  = .03) and a main effect of condition on negative emotions, F (1, 203) = 26.29, p  < .001, η p 2  = .11 (see Table 2 for descriptive statistics). Importantly, as predicted, the interaction between condition and gender was significant both on negative, F (1, 203) = 12.83, p  < .001, η p 2  = .05, and positive, F (1, 203) = 9.03, p  = .003, η p 2  = .04, emotions. Specifically in line with Hypothesis 3a, as shown in Table 2 , women reported significantly more negative emotions toward sexualized female ads than neutral ads ( p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = 1.15) whereas men’s negative emotions did not vary across conditions ( p  = .296). Moreover, in the sexualized condition women showed more negative emotions than men ( p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = .64) whereas this comparison was not significant in the control condition ( p  = .316). In addition, disconfirming Prediction 3b, women showed lower positive emotions in the sexualized female ads than in the neutral ads condition ( p  = .003, Cohen’s d  = .62) whereas men’s positive emotions did not differ across conditions ( p  = .179). Moreover, women exposed to sexualized female ads manifested lower positive emotions than men ( p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = .75) whereas this difference was not significant in the control condition ( p  = .754).

Concerning emotional reactions toward male model ads, univariate analyses on positive emotions showed no effects of gender, condition, or Gender x Condition interaction, Fs (2, 203) < 1.94, ps >  .165, η p 2 s < .008), thus disconfirming Hypothesis 7a. On the contrary, as predicted by Hypothesis 7b, a main effect of condition emerged on negative emotions, F (1, 203) = 16.23, p  < .001, η p 2  = .07 (see Table 2 for descriptive statistics). Importantly, this effect was qualified by a significant Gender x Condition interaction, F (1, 203) = 6.06, p  = .015, η p 2  = .03. As shown in Table 2 , women reported significantly more negative emotions after exposure to sexualized male ads than neutral ads ( p  < .001, Cohen’s d  = 1.01) whereas men’s negative emotions did not differ across conditions ( p  = .290). Moreover, unexpectedly, in the control condition men showed more negative emotions than women, ( p  = .006, Cohen’s d  = .62) whereas this comparison was not significant in the sexualized condition ( p  = .450).

We computed two separate overall indices of emotional negativity for ads with female and male models by subtracting participants’ responses on positive emotions from those on negative emotions. A series of moderated mediation analyses were performed through PROCESS (Model n° 8, Hayes 2013 ) to test Hypothesis 4 and replicate Study 3’s results. In the first model we included condition (control = 0, sexualized = 1) as the independent variable, emotional negativity toward female ads (continuous, centered) as the mediator, participants’ gender (men = 0, women = 1) as the moderator, and product attractiveness toward female ads as the outcome. The overall model was significant, ( R 2  = .41), F (4, 202) = 35.07, p  < .001. In line with Study 3’s results, a significant negative indirect effect of condition through emotional negativity emerged for women ( b  = −.70, SE  = .14, 95% CI [−1.00, −.44], with 5000 bootstrap samples). Therefore, sexualized (vs. neutral) female ads increased women’s negative emotions, which in turn decreased product attractiveness. This pattern was not found among men ( b  = −.04, SE  = .14, 95% CI [−.25, .31]).

To further test Hypothesis 4 and replicate Study 3’s results, the same analysis was conducted on purchase intentions toward female ads. The overall model was significant ( R 2  = .40), F (4, 202) = 33.58, p  < .001. Replicating Study 3’s findings, the negative indirect effect of condition through emotional negativity was significant for women ( b  = −.70, SE  = .14, 95% CI [−1.00, −.44], with 5000 bootstrap samples). In other words, as for product attractiveness, women reported lower purchase intentions after viewing sexualized female model ads than neutral ads because of their higher level of negative emotions. Again, this pattern was not observed among men ( b  = −.04, SE  = .13, 95% CI [−.23, .30]).

Concerning reactions toward ad with male models, we ran the same analyses conducted for female ads with the difference that emotional negativity toward male model ads was the mediator and product attractiveness and purchase intentions toward male model ads were considered separately as outcome variables. The overall model on male ads’ product attractiveness was significant ( R 2  = .45), F (4, 202) = 40.91, p  < .001. Importantly, a negative indirect effect of condition through emotional negativity emerged for women ( b  = −.52, SE  = .12, 95% CI [−.78, −.27] with 5000 bootstrap samples). Therefore, as for sexualized female model ads, exposure to sexualized (vs. neutral) male model ads increased women’s negative emotions, which in turn decreased product attractiveness. This mediation was not found among male participants ( b  = −.11, SE  = .13, 95% CI [−.38, .13]).

Finally, the overall model on purchase intentions in response to ads with male models was also significant ( R 2  = .38), F (4, 202) = 30.90, p  < .001 and a negative indirect effect of condition through emotional negativity emerged again only for women ( b  = −.48, SE  = .12, 95% CI [−.73, −.25], with 5000 bootstrap samples). In line with the results for product attractiveness, women exposed to ads with sexualized (vs. neutral) male models reported higher emotional negativity, which in turn decreased their purchase intentions. Again, this pattern did not emerge for men ( b  = −.11, SE  = .12, 95% CI [−.35, .12]).

The current study has shown several important results that substantially extend previous research. First, in line with Hypothesis 6 as well as Wirtz et al. ( 2018 ), both men and women reported lower attractiveness and purchase intentions toward products advertised by sexualized male models than neutral ads. Second, supporting findings of Studies 1–3 and Hypothesis 1b, women still showed significantly lower product attractiveness and purchase intentions toward sexualized (vs. neutral) female model ads. Also, in line with Study 1 and Study 3, men’s product attractiveness and purchase intentions toward ads with female models did not vary across conditions, again disconfirming Hypothesis 1a.

Third, in line with Hypothesis 7b, another important result of the present study is that participants showed higher negative emotions toward male sexualized ads compared to neutral ads. Importantly, this effect was stronger for women than men participants. Fourth, extending Study 3’s results, in line with Hypothesis 3a and disconfirming Prediction 3b, women showed higher negative emotions and lower positive emotions after exposure to female sexualized than neutral ads. Therefore, extending Study 3’s results, female participants negatively reacted to sexualization in advertising, regardless of whether the depicted body was male or female. On the other hand, it is worth noting that, extending Study 3’s results and contrary to Hypothesis 7a and Hypothesis 7b, men’s emotions toward ads with male and female models did not vary across conditions.

Most importantly, mediation analyses confirmed and extended Study 3’s results and Hypothesis 4 by showing that, regardless of the models’ gender, sexualized (vs. neutral) ads increased women’s emotional negativity, which in turn decreased attractiveness and purchase intentions toward the products. All in all, the present research shows that in advertising women overall dislike the use of sexualized images whereas men are indifferent to female sexualized models and respond negatively to male sexualized ads.

Meta-Analysis

To further analyze how women and men reacted to female sexualization in advertising, following the procedure by Riva et al. ( 2015 ), we meta-analytically combined results of Studies 1–4. Concerning women’s reactions, results were consistent across the four studies showing lower product attractiveness (in line with Hypothesis 1b) and purchase intentions after exposure to sexualized female model than neutral ads. The meta-analysis showed that the weight-combined Z -score for condition (sexualized vs. control) was statistically significant both on women’s product attractiveness ( Z  = 10.08, p  < .001) and purchase intentions ( Z  = 9.40, p  < .001). The effect size regarding women’s lower product attractiveness in the sexualized than control condition was large ( d  = 1.04, η p 2  = .21); likewise, women’s lower purchase intentions in the sexualized than control condition was large ( d  = .96, η p 2  = .19). Concerning men’s reactions, in the present four studies men were shown to be basically indifferent to exposure to sexualized female models compared to neutral ads. Strengthening our argument, contrary to Hypothesis 1a, meta-analysis results showed that for men the condition (sexualized vs. control) was not statistically significant both on product attractiveness ( Z  = 2.52, p  = .064, d  = .25, η p 2  = .02) and on purchase intentions ( Z  = 1.70, p  = .243, d  = .17, η p 2  = .01).

General Discussion

The present research showed a series of important results. First, across the four studies we have confirmed Hypothesis 1b, namely that women were less attracted toward products and had lower purchase intentions when they were presented with sexualized female models than with neutral ads. Second, and disconfirming Hypothesis 1a, men, contrary to women, were largely unaffected by the level of female sexualization of the ads. These results are further supported by the meta-analysis, which provides a reliable and trustworthy pattern of cumulative evidence.

In addition, contrary to Hypothesis 2, in Study 2 participants’ attitudes that view women as sexual objects and men as sex-driven were not related to their reactions toward the female model ads. Moreover, both in Study 3 and Study 4 and in line with Hypothesis 3a, women reported higher negative emotions after exposure to female sexualized than neutral ads. However, partially disconfirming Prediction 3b, women’s positive emotions varied across conditions in Study 4 but not in Study 3. In addition, in line with the lack of effects on product attractiveness and purchase intentions, men’s emotions were never affected by condition.

Most importantly, consistent with Hypothesis 4, in Study 3 women’s negative emotions toward sexualized female (vs. neutral) ads were found to be one mechanism underlying their decrement on product attractiveness and purchase intentions. In addition, in line with Hypothesis 5b, Study 3 also demonstrated hostile sexism as one individual difference that moderated purchase intentions: Higher hostile sexism in men was associated with higher purchase intentions after viewing sexualized female ads than neutral ads. Moreover, hostile sexism predicted higher purchase intentions among women in the control condition.

Moreover, in line with Hypothesis 6, in Study 4 both men and women expressed lower product attractiveness and purchase intentions toward sexualized male model ads than neutral ads. In addition, partially confirming Hypothesis 7b, women showed higher negative emotions toward male sexualized ads compared to neutral ads, whereas men’s emotions did not vary. Importantly, in Study 4 we extended the mediation analysis to male model ads by showing that women’s negative emotions were responsible for the decrement on product attractiveness and purchase intentions toward both female and male models sexualized (vs. neutral) ads.

Overall, our findings on product attractiveness and purchase intentions substantially advance Wirtz et al.’s ( 2018 ) results by showing that female sexualization in advertising has a negative effect on women’s responses and has a null effect on men’s responses and that the use of male sexualization is counterproductive both for women and men. Concerning male model ads, this pattern of results contributes to Wirtz et al.’s analyses because it clearly demonstrates that not only men, but also women, dislike male sexualization in advertisement, in contrast with Jones et al.’s ( 1998 ) claims. Concerning female model ads, our pattern of results is in contrast with Wirtz et al.’s conclusions regarding men’s higher attractiveness toward sexualized female ads and women’s lack of effect on purchase intentions. One possibility to explain this discrepancy is the fact that Wirtz et al.’s meta-analysis includes studies starting from the early 1970s and the advertising context nowadays might be different. Indeed, in the last decade, the femvertising movement for body-positive advertising has emerged and new ad campaigns using empowerment messages to women were created (Castillo 2014 ; Teng et al. 2020 ). Therefore, in the last decade people may have developed an appreciation for a variety of female and male model ads that goes beyond sexualization, a possibility that would help explain our participants’ mostly negative reactions toward sexualized ads.

Another important finding of the present study is that exposure to sexualized ads significantly impacts women’s emotions. These findings enrich an under-investigated area of research. Indeed, although some studies indicated that consumers who purchase new products are more likely to form preferences (favorable or unfavorable) based on affective evaluations (Muehling and McCann 1993 ; Reichert 2002 ), research that analyzes advertisement-related emotions within the context of sexualization is scarce. Therefore, a significant theoretical contribution of the present study is our moderated mediation analyses, which suggest that negative emotions can work as one mechanism that regulates women’s reactions. Overall, sexualized images work against women’s product attractiveness and purchase intentions because they elicit negative emotions.

Moreover, the present study deepens our understanding of the moderating role of individual differences in gender attitudes on the relation between female ad sexualization and purchase intentions: Especially men with higher hostile sexism showed more purchase intentions after viewing sexualized than neutral ads. This finding nicely parallels results by Zawisza et al. ( 2018 ) who showed a positive association between hostile sexism and purchase intentions toward stereotypically feminine ads. In addition, our results suggest that the endorsement of hostile sexism by men may favor the validation of sexualized female models proposed by media. Given that the exposure to female sexualized images increases hostile sexism (Fox and Potocki 2016 ; Rollero 2013 ), our results complement this evidence and suggest a vicious circle between female sexualization and hostile sexism. Future research should further investigate this possibility and test whether it is specific to hostile sexism or it may also extend to benevolent sexism, a construct that was not measured in the present study. An additional unexpected result was that the higher women’s hostile sexism, the higher their purchase intentions in the control condition. This result suggests a general relation between women’s level of hostile sexism and consumerism, a possibility that should be further investigated in future research.

Limitations and Future Research Directions

The present research presents some limitations. In line with previous literature we have tested the role of the gender-relevance of the products on product attractiveness and purchase intentions and found no significant effects. However, the products chosen were not varied in a systematic way with respect to other characteristics. For example, some products were gendered in a way that may make them less appealing to female consumers (e.g., men’s shoes), thus creating a potential confound leading to the decrease in women’s preferences. However, the gender of our participants did not affect the results in the control condition, which helps exclude the possibility of such a confound. Nevertheless, it would still be important for future studies to systematically vary the products’ gender target. Also related to this point, future research may assess participants’ relationship status, a variable that we did not assess and that may further modulate participants’ responses because some products may be interesting to buy for one’s partner. In addition, the economic value of the products (luxury vs. inexpensive) was not systematically varied; future research may be conducted to ascertain whether this feature may also modulate consumers’ responses toward sexualized versus neutral ads.

In all four studies, we compared sexualized ads including (fe)male models in revealing clothing to neutral ads including the same product as in the sexualized condition, but devoid of the model. To have a further control condition, we suggest future studies also have the same (fe)male models but portrayed in non-sexualized ways. More generally, we think that Study 4’s results on the effects of male sexualized models are promising especially because they demonstrate that the mediating role of women’s negative emotions nicely parallel results obtained toward female sexualized models. However, our study did not provide any information on the reasons why men responded unfavorably to sexualized male model ads. One speculation is that male ad sexualization confronts men with their explicit or implicit homophobia. To explore this possibility, future research may further investigate men’s reactions toward male sexualized ads by also assessing homophobia and masculinity norms.

Finally, another future direction of the present study is to diversify the type of models included in the ads. The present study was conducted in Italy and all models were White and reflected the sexualized thin ideal for women and the muscular ideal for men. Therefore, we suggest more diversity in future studies.

Practice Implications

The present study presents several practical implications. Concerning marketing, our results are at odds with current sexualizing marketing strategies, which are based on the assumption that “sex sells.” Indeed, our findings suggest that, at the marketing level, the use of female sexualization in advertising is counterproductive for women and useless for men as consumers and that the use of male sexualization is counterproductive both for women and men. Put differently, our findings show that “sex does not sell,” a result that questions sexualization as a useful marketing strategy.

Concerning ethical implications, the present findings complement a large amount of research based on objectification theory that has shown detrimental effects of exposure to media sexualization on women’s and men’s well-being (Agliata and Tantleff-Dunn 2004 ; Leit et al. 2002 ; Lorenzen et al. 2004 ; Ward 2016 ). Therefore, considering the psychological damage and the practical inefficacy of sexualized ads, we argue that sexualization in advertising should be addressed in public policy discourse. This issue is particularly relevant with respect to sexualized advertisement aimed at children (Pacilli et al. 2016 ), which would require even stricter regulations. Also relevant to public policy, media literacy programs may be employed to buffer the negative effects of media sexualization (see Guizzo and Cadinu 2020 ; Tylka and Augustus-Horvath 2011 ).

Conclusions

The present set of studies substantially extends previous research by repeatedly demonstrating that the “sex sells” approach should not be taken for granted. Indeed, we have shown an overall pattern of results that strongly contradicts current sexualizing marketing strategies: Women reacted negatively to both female and male sexualized ads by expressing higher negative emotions, which in turn disinclined them to purchase these products. On the other hand, men did not show any significant increment either on product attractiveness or purchase intentions toward female sexualized compared to neutral ads, and they also reacted negatively to male sexualization in ads. Therefore, the use of sexualization in advertisement seems paradoxical because our findings demonstrate that sexualized ads may backfire regarding their final aim, which is to sell products. These results should lead advertising agencies to wonder whether the proliferation of these ads is justified.

Agliata, D., & Tantleff-Dunn, S. (2004). The impact of media exposure on males’ body image. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 23 , 7–22. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.23.1.7.26988 .

Article   Google Scholar  

Albarello, F., & Rubini, M. (2012). Reducing dehumanisation outcomes towards Blacks: The role of multiple categorisation and of humanity identity. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42 , 875–882. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.1902 .

Aylesworth, A. B., Goodstein, R. C., & Kalra, A. (1999). Effect of archetypal embeds on feelings: An indirect route to affecting attitudes? Journal of Advertising, 28 , 73–81. https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.1999.10673590 .

Bartky, S. L. (1990). Femininity and domination: Studies in the phenomenology of oppression . New York: Routledge.

Google Scholar  

Behm-Morawitz, E. (2017). Examining the intersection of race and gender in video game advertising. Journal of Marketing Communications, 23 , 220–239. https://doi.org/10.1080/13527266.2014.914562 .

Belch, M. A., Holgerson, B. E., Belch, G. E., & Koppman, J. (1982). Psychophysical and cognitive responses to sex in advertising. In A. A. Mitchell (Ed.), Advances in consumer research (pp. 424–427). Ann Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer Research.

Bello, D. C., Pitts, P. E., & Etzel, M. J. (1983). The communication effects of controversial sexual content in television programs and commercials. Journal of Advertising, 12 , 32–42. https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.1983.10672846 .

Black, I. R., & Morton, P. (2017). Appealing to men and women using sexual appeals in advertising: In the battle of the sexes, is a truce possible? Journal of Marketing Communications, 23 , 331–350. https://doi.org/10.1080/13527266.2015.1015108 .

Bongiorno, R., Bain, P. G., & Haslam, N. (2013). When sex doesn't sell: Using sexualized images of women reduces support for ethical campaigns. PLoS One, 8 , e83311. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0083311 .

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Bushman, B. J. (2005). Violence and sex in television programs do not sell products in advertisements. Psychological Science, 16 , 702–708. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01599.x .

Castillo, M. (2014). These stats prove femvertising works . Retrieved from http://www.adweek.com/news/technology/these-stats-prove-femvertising-works-160704 .

Chen, Z., Fiske, S. T., & Lee, T. L. (2009). Ambivalent sexism and power-related gender-role ideology in marriage. Sex Roles, 60 , 765–778. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-009-9585-9 .

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Conley, T. D., & Ramsey, L. R. (2011). Killing us softly? Investigating portrayals of women and men in contemporary magazine advertisements. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 35 , 469–478. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684311413383 .

Dafferner, M., Campagna, J., & Rodgers, R. F. (2019). Making gains: Hypermuscularity and objectification of male and female Olympic athletes in sports illustrated across 60 years. Body Image, 29 , 156–160. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.04.001 .

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Dahl, D. W., Sengupta, J., & Vohs, K. D. (2009). Sex in advertising: Gender differences and the role of relationship commitment. Journal of Consumer Research, 36 , 215–231. https://doi.org/10.1086/597158 .

Dillard, J. P., & Pfau, M. (2002). The persuasion handbook: Developments in theory and practice . Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Dillard, J. P., & Wilson, B. J. (1993). Communication and affect: Thoughts, feelings, and issues for the future. Communication Research, 20 , 637–646. https://doi.org/10.1177/009365093020005001 .

Dudley, S. C. (1999). Consumer attitudes toward nudity in advertising. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 79 , 89–96. https://doi.org/10.1080/10696679.1999.11501854 .

Ferguson, C. J., Cruz, A. M., Martinez, D., Rueda, S. M., & Ferguson, D. E. (2010). Violence and sex as advertising strategies in television commercials. European Psychologist, 15 , 304–311. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000016 .

Fox, J., & Potocki, B. (2016). Lifetime video game consumption, interpersonal aggression, hostile sexism, and rape myth acceptance: A cultivation perspective. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 31 , 1912–1931. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260515570747 .

Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T.-A. (1997). Objectification theory: Toward understanding women’s lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21 , 173–206. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1997.tb00108.x .

Giaccardi, S., Cooper, R., Heldman, C., Cooper-Jones, N., McTaggart, N., Juliano, L., … Conroy, M. (2019). Bias and inclusion in advertising: An analysis of 2018 Cannes Lions Film Craft ads. The Geena Davis Institute on Gender in Media at Mount Saint Mary’s University. Retrieved from https://seejane.org/research-informs-empowers/bias-inclusion-in-advertising-an-analysis-of-2018-cannes-lions-film-craft-ads/ .

Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1996). The ambivalent sexism inventory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70 , 491–512.

Grabe, S., Ward, L. M., & Hyde, J. S. (2008). The role of the media in body image concerns among women: A meta-analysis of experimental and correlational studies. Psychological Bulletin, 134 , 460–476. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.460 .

Grazer, W. F., & Kessling, G. (1995). The effect of print advertising’s use of sexual themes on brand recall and purchase intention: A product specific investigation of male responses. Journal of Applied Business Research, 11 , 47–58. https://doi.org/10.19030/jabr.v11i3.5859 .

Guastini, M., Cosenza, G., Colombari, J., & Gasparri E. (2014). Come la pubblicità racconta le donne e gli uomini, in Italia [How advertising represents women and men in Italy]. Report of the Italian art directors Club. Retrieved from https://giovannacosenza.files.wordpress.com/2014/11/come-la-pubblicitacc80-racconta-gli-italiani.pdf .

Guizzo, F., & Cadinu, M. (2020). Women, not objects: Testing a sensitizing web campaign against female sexual objectification to temper sexual harassment and hostile sexism. Media Psychology . Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2020.1756338 , 1, 29.

Harker, M., Harker, D., & Svensen, S. (2005). Attitudes towards gender portrayal in advertising: An Australian perspective. Journal of Marketing Management, 21 , 251–264. https://doi.org/10.1362/0267257053166820ùù .

Hatton, E., & Trautner, M. N. (2011). Equal opportunity objectification? The sexualization of men and women on the cover of rolling stone. Sexuality and Culture, 15 , 256–278. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12119-011-9093-2 .

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach . New York, NJ: Guilford Press.

Huang, M. H. (2004). Romantic love and sex: Their relationship and impacts on ad attitudes. Psychology & Marketing, 21 , 53–73. https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.10115 .

Jones, M. Y., Stanaland, A. J. S., & Gelb, B. D. (1998). Beefcake and cheesecake: Insights for advertisers. Journal of Advertising, 27 , 33–51. https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.1998.10673551 .

LaTour, M. S. (1990). Female nudity in print advertising: An analysis of gender differences in arousal and ad response. Psychology & Marketing, 7 , 65–81.

LaTour, M. S., & Henthorne, T. L. (1994). Ethical judgments of sexual appeals in print advertising. Journal of Advertising, 23 , 81–90. https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.1994.10673453 .

Leit, R. A., Gray, J. J., & Pope Jr., H. G. (2002). The media’s representation of the ideal male body: A cause for muscle dysmorphia? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 31 , 334–338. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.10019 .

Lorenzen, L. A., Grieve, F. G., & Thomas, A. (2004). Exposure to male models decreases men’s body satisfaction. Sex Roles, 51 , 743–748. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-004-0723-0 .

Loughnan, S., & Pacilli, M. G. (2014). Seeing (and treating) others as sexual objects: Toward a more complete mapping of sexual objectification. TPM: Testing, Psychometrics, Methodology in Applied Psychology, 21 , 309–325. https://doi.org/10.4473/TPM21.3.6 .

Lull, R. B., & Bushman, B. J. (2015). Do sex and violence sell? A meta-analytic review of the effects of sexual and violent media and ad content on memory, attitudes, and buying intentions. Psychological Bullettin, 141 , 1022–1048. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000018 .

Manganelli Rattazzi, A., Volpato, C., & Canova, L. (2008). L’atteggiamento ambivalente verso donne e uomini: Un contributo alla validazione delle scale ASI e AMI [The ambivalent attitude toward women and men: A contribution to the validation of the ASI and AMI scales]. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 35 , 261–287.

Mittal, B., & Lassar, W. M. (2000). Sexual liberalism as a determinant of consumer response to sex in advertising. Journal of Business and Psychology, 15 , 111–127. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007723003376 .

Muehling, D. D., & McCann, M. (1993). Attitude toward the ad: A review. Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 15 , 25–58. https://doi.org/10.1080/10641734.1993.10505002 .

Nussbaum, M. C. (1995). Objectification. Philosophy & Public Affairs, 24 , 249–291. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1088-4963.1995.tb00032.x .

Pacilli, M. G., Tomasetto, C., & Cadinu, M. (2016). Exposure to sexualized advertisements disrupts children’s math performance by reducing working memory. Sex Roles, 74 , 389–398. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0581-6 .

Paek, H. J., Nelson, M. R., & Vilela, A. M. (2011). Examination of gender-role portrayals in television advertising across seven countries. Sex Roles, 64 , 192–207. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-010-9850-y .

Pan, P. L. (2014). Toward an integrated model of purchase intention of dietary supplements in sexually oriented advertising. Journal of Food Products Marketing, 20 , 132–145. https://doi.org/10.1080/10454446.2012.726948 .

Peterson, R. A., & Kerin, R. A. (1977). The female role in advertisements: Some experimental evidence. Journal of Marketing, 41 , 59–63. https://doi.org/10.1177/002224297704100407 .

Pope Jr., H. G., Olivardia, R., Borowiecki III, J. J., & Cohane, G. H. (2001). The growing commercial value of the male body: A longitudinal survey of advertising in women's magazines. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 70 , 189–192. https://doi.org/10.1159/000056252 .

Putrevu, S. (2008). Consumer responses toward sexual and nonsexual appeals: The influence of involvement, need for cognition (NFC), and gender. Journal of Advertising, 37 , 57–69. https://doi.org/10.2753/JOA0091-3367370205 .

Reichert, T. (2002). Sex in advertising research: A review of content, effects, and functions of sexual information in consumer advertising. Annual Review of Sex Research, 13 , 241–273. https://doi.org/10.1080/10532528.2002.10559806 .

Reichert, T., Heckler, S. E., & Jackson, S. (2001). The effects of sexual social marketing appeals on cognitive processing and persuasion. Journal of Advertising, 30 , 13–27. https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.2001.10673628 .

Reichert, T., LaTour, M. S., & Kim, J. Y. (2007). Assessing the influence of gender and sexual self-schema on affective responses to sexual content in advertising. Journal of Current Issues & Research in Advertising, 29 , 63–77. https://doi.org/10.1080/10641734.2007.10505217 .

Reidenbach, R. E., & McCleary, K. W. (1983). Advertising and male nudity: An experimental investigation. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 11 , 444–454. https://doi.org/10.1177/009207038301100407 .

Riva, P., Brambilla, M., & Vaes, J. (2015). Bad guys suffer less (social pain): Moral status influences judgements of others’ social suffering. British Journal of Social Psychology, 55 , 88–108. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12114 .

Rohlinger, A. R. (2002). Eroticizing men: Cultural influences on advertising and male objectification. Sex Roles, 46 , 61–74. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1016575909173ù .

Rollero, C. (2013). Men and women facing objectification: The effects of media models on well-being, self-esteem and ambivalent sexism. Revista De Psicología Social: International Journal of Social Psychology, 28 , 373–382. https://doi.org/10.1174/021347413807719166 .

Sengupta, J., & Dahl, D. W. (2008). Gender-related reactions to gratuitous sex appeals in advertising. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 18 , 62–78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2007.10.010 .

Simpson, P., Horton, S., & Brown, G. (1996). Male nudity in advertisements: A modified replication and extension of gender and product effects. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Sciences, 24 , 257–262. https://doi.org/10.1177/0092070396243006 .

Soley, L., & Kurzbard, G. (1986). Sex in advertising: A comparison of 1964 and 1984 magazine advertisements. Journal of Advertising, 15 , 46–54. https://doi.org/10.1080/00913367.1986.10673018 .

Stankiewicz, J. M., & Rosselli, F. (2008). Women as sex objects and victims in print advertisements. Sex Roles, 58 , 579–589. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-007-9359-1 .

Teng, F., Hu, J., Chen, Z., Poon, K. T., & Bai, Y. (2020). Sexism and the effectiveness of femvertising in China: A corporate social responsibility perspective. Sex Roles. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-020-01164-8 .

Tylka, T. L., & Augustus-Horvath, C. L. (2011). Fighting self- objectification in prevention and intervention contexts. In R. Calogero, S. Tantleff-Dunn, & J. Thompson (Eds.), Self-objectification in women: Causes, consequences, and counteractions (pp. 187–214). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Vaes, J., Paladino, P., Castelli, L., Leyens, J. P., & Giovanazzi, A. (2003). On the behavioral consequences of infrahumanization: The implicit role of uniquely human emotions in intergroup relations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85 , 1016–1034. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1016 .

Verhellen, Y., Dens, N., & De Pelsmacker, P. (2016). A longitudinal content analysis of gender role portrayal in Belgian television advertising. Journal of Marketing Communications, 22 , 170–188. https://doi.org/10.1080/13527266.2013.871321 .

Ward, L. M. (2002). Does television exposure affect emerging adults’ attitudes and assumptions about sexual relationships? Correlational and experimental confirmation. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 31 , 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1014068031532 .

Ward, L. M. (2016). Media and sexualization: State of empirical research, 1995-2015. The Journal of Sex Research, 53 , 560–577. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2016.1142496 .

Wirtz, J. G., Sparks, J. V., & Zimbres, T. M. (2018). The effect of exposure to sexual appeals in advertisements on memory, attitude, and purchase intention: A meta-analytic review. International Journal of Advertising, 37 , 168–198. https://doi.org/10.1080/02650487.2017.1334996 .

Wyllie, J., Carlson, J., & Rosenberger III, P. J. (2015). Does sexual-stimuli intensity and sexual self-schema influence female consumers’ reactions toward sexualised advertising? An Australian perspective. Australasian Marketing Journal, 23 , 188–195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ausmj.2015.06.004 .

Zawisza, M., & Cinnirella, M. (2010). What matters more—Breaking tradition or stereotype content? Envious and paternalistic gender stereotypes and advertising effectiveness. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 40 , 1767–1797. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00639.x .

Zawisza, M., Luyt, R., Zawadzka, A. M., & Buczny, J. (2018). Cross-cultural sexism and the effectiveness of gender (non) traditional advertising: A comparison of purchase intentions in Poland, South Africa, and the United Kingdom. Sex Roles, 79 , 738–751. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-018-0906-8 .

Download references

Open access funding provided by Università degli Studi di Padova within the CRUI-CARE Agreement.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Developmental and Socialization Psychology, University of Padova, Via Venezia 8, 35131, Padova, Italy

Sarah Gramazio, Mara Cadinu & Francesca Guizzo

Department of Life Sciences, University of Trieste, Trieste, Italy

Andrea Carnaghi

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Francesca Guizzo .

Ethics declarations

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of APA ethical guidelines and the ethical committee of the University of Padova. All participants signed a written informed consent.

Additional information

Publisher’s note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Esm 1 (docx 159 kb), rights and permissions.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Gramazio, S., Cadinu, M., Guizzo, F. et al. Does Sex Really Sell? Paradoxical Effects of Sexualization in Advertising on Product Attractiveness and Purchase Intentions. Sex Roles 84 , 701–719 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-020-01190-6

Download citation

Accepted : 08 September 2020

Published : 23 September 2020

Issue Date : June 2021

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-020-01190-6

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Sexualization
  • Advertising
  • Purchase intentions
  • Product attractiveness
  • Emotional responses
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

Pardon Our Interruption

As you were browsing something about your browser made us think you were a bot. There are a few reasons this might happen:

  • You've disabled JavaScript in your web browser.
  • You're a power user moving through this website with super-human speed.
  • You've disabled cookies in your web browser.
  • A third-party browser plugin, such as Ghostery or NoScript, is preventing JavaScript from running. Additional information is available in this support article .

To regain access, please make sure that cookies and JavaScript are enabled before reloading the page.

Advertisement

Supported by

Inside the List

Sex Sells. It’s True Now and It Was True 100 Years Ago.

  • Share full article

sex sells essay

By Elisabeth Egan

  • Dec. 26, 2019

BLAST FROM THE PAST We don’t have a crystal ball so we can’t look into the future to see what will be on the best-seller list in 2020. What we do have at the Book Review is the ability to look back — either in our library of bound issues dating back to 1930 or in TimesPast, a handy digital tool that takes us back to the eras when O. Henry was carousing at Pete’s Tavern and Dorothy Parker was holding court at the Algonquin.

I was curious to find out what mere mortals were reading and writing 100 years ago, so into the archives I went. In the books pages from the Jan. 4, 1920, edition of The Times, among ads for writing coaches and diet books (“Why Grow Fat?”) — some things never change — I stumbled on “What the New Year Will Bring in Books.” The unbylined article relied on the expertise of Sir Ernest Hodder Williams, president of the London publishing firm Hodder & Stoughton and vice president of the George H. Doran Company in New York, who declared “the coming twelvemonth” to be “‘the biggest year ever’ in the reading of books.”

“In these times that follow the war, people are reading fiction to ‘get away’ from the drabness of everyday existence,” said Sir Ernest. “I believe that fiction will mean more to the public than it ever has.” He described the most popular novels of the time: the “open air” story that took “city clerks and other busy indoor workers into a broad, open outdoor life” and the domestic one that dealt “with young people in a bright and pleasant way.” He also said, “The detective novel and the novel of sheer adventure are always popular.” As for futuristic fiction and stories of war, Sir Ernest did not have high hopes for their success: “People have lived so close to life, to bigness and reality in these past years that they are not interested in reading theories. They want facts.”

He continued, “One thing I do look for, and that is a tremendous flood of sex novels. I think that is inevitable. I think myself that the new candor will be, in the long run, a good thing for society. ... The old secrecy in relation to sex can never come back. Moreover, the sexes are franker and more friendly with each other than they have ever been before. Men and women have worked and suffered side by side through all these hard years.”

What would Sir Ernest make of the fact that “ Fifty Shades of Grey ” was the best-selling book of the last decade in the United States? Hard to say, but interesting to think about. After all, everything old is new again.

Elisabeth Egan is an editor at the Book Review and the author of “A Window Opens.”

Follow New York Times Books on Facebook , Twitter and Instagram , s ign up for our newsletter or our literary calendar . And listen to us on the Book Review podcast .

Jesse Marczyk Ph.D.

Understanding Sex in Advertising

Getting people to look or buy.

Posted June 26, 2017 | Reviewed by Devon Frye

  • The Fundamentals of Sex
  • Take our Romantic Personality Test
  • Find a sex therapist near me

When people post videos on YouTube, one major point of interest for content creators and aggregators is to capture as much attention as possible. Your video is adrift in a sea of information and you're trying to get as many eyes or clicks on your work as possible.

In that realm, first impressions are all-important: you want your video to have an attention-grabbing thumbnail image, as that will likely be the only thing viewers see before they actually click (or don't) on it. So how do people go about capturing attention in that realm? One popular method is to ensure their thumbnail has a very emotive expression on it; a face of shock, embarrassment , stress , or any similar emotion . That's certainly one way of attracting attention: trying to convince people there is something worth looking at, not unlike articles titled along the lines of five shocking tips for a better sex life (and number 3 will blow your mind!).

Speaking of sex, that's another popular method of grabbing attention: it's fairly common for video thumbnails to feature people or body parts in various stages of undress. Not much will pull eyes towards a video like the promise of sex (and if you're feeling an urge to click on that link, you'll have experienced exactly what I'm talking about).

Jesse Marczyk

If sex happens to be attention-grabbing, the natural question arises concerning what you might do with that attention once you have it. Much of the time, that answer will involve selling some good or service. In other words, sex is used as a form of advertising to try and sell things. "If you enjoyed that picture of a woman wearing a thong, you'll surely love our reasonably-priced laptops!" Something along those lines, anyway.

Provided that's your goal, lots of questions naturally start to crop up: How effective is sex at these goals ? Does it capture attention well? Does it help people notice and remember your product or brand? Are those who viewed your sexy advert more likely to buy the product you're selling? How do other factors, like the gender of the person viewing the ad, contribute to your success in these realms?

These are some of the questions examined in a recent meta-analysis by Wirtz, Sparks, and Zimbres (2017). The researchers searched the literature and found about 80 studies, representing about 18,000 participants. They sought to find out what effects featuring sexually provocative material had, on average (defined in terms of style of dress, sexual behavior, innuendo, or sexual embeds, which is where hidden messages or images are placed within the ad, like the word "sex" added somewhere to the picture, which is something people apparently think is a good idea sometimes). These ads had to have been compared with a comparable, non-sexual ad for the same product to be included in the analysis to determine which was more effective.

The effectiveness of these ads was assessed across a number of domains as well, including ad recognition (in aided and unaided contexts), whether the brand being advertised in the ad could be recalled (i.e., were people paying attention to just the sex, or did they remember the product?), the positive or negative response people had to the ad, what people thought about the brand being advertised with sex, and whether the ad actually got them interested in purchasing the product (does sex sell?).

Finally, a number of potentially moderating factors that might influence these effects were considered. The first of these was gender: did these ads have different impacts on men and women? Other factors included the gender of the model used in the advertisement, the date the article was published (to see if attitudes shifted over time), the sample used (college students or not), and, most interestingly, product/ad congruity: did the type of product being advertised matter when it came to whether sex was effective? Perhaps sex might help sell a product like sun-tan lotion (as the beach might be a good place to pick up mates), but be much less effective for selling, say, laptops.

In terms of capturing attention, sex works. Of the 20 effects looking at the recall for ads, the average size was d = .38. Interesting, this effect was slightly larger for the congruent ads (d = .45), but completely reversed for the incongruent ones (d = -.45). Sex was good at getting people to remember ads selling a sex-related product, but not just generally useful. That said, they seemed better at getting people to remember just the ads . When the researchers turned to the matter of whether the brands within the ads were more likely to be recalled, the 31 effects looking at brand recognition turned out to barely break zero (d = .09). While sex might be attention-grabbing, it didn't seem especially good at getting people to remember the objects being sold.

Regarding people's attitudes towards the ads, sex seems like something of a wash (d = -.07). Digging a little deeper revealed a more nuanced picture of these reactions, though: while sexual ads seemed to be a modest hit with the men (d = .27), they had the opposite effect on women (d = -.38). Women seemed to dislike the ads modestly more than men liked them, as sexual strategies theory would suggest (for the record, the type of model being depicted didn't make much of a difference. In order, people liked males models the least (d = -.28), then female models (d = -.20), and couples were mildly positive, d = .08).

sex sells essay

Curiously, both the men and women seemed to be in agreement regarding their stance towards brands that used sex to sell things: negative, on the whole (d - =.22). For women, this makes some intuitive sense: they didn't seem to be a fan of the sexual ads, so they weren't exactly feeling too ingratiated towards the brand itself.

But why were the men negatively inclined towards the brand if they were favorably inclined towards the ads? I can only speculate on that front, but I assume it would have something to do with their inevitable disappointment: either that the brands were promising on sex the male customers likely knew they couldn't deliver on, or perhaps the men simply wanted to enjoy the sex part and the brand itself ended up getting in their way. I can't imagine men would be too happy with their porn time being interrupted by an ad for toilet paper or fruit snacks mid-video.

Finally, turning the matter of purchase intentions—whether the ads encouraged people to want to buy the product or not—it seemed that sex didn't really sell, but it didn't really seem to hurt, either (d = .01). One interesting exception in that realm was that sex appeals were actually less likely to get people to buy a product when the product being sold was incongruent with the sexual appeal (d = -.24). Putting that into a simple example, the phrase "strip club buffet" probably doesn't whet many appetites, and wouldn't be a strong selling point for such a venue. Sex can be something of a disease vector, and associating your food with that might illicit more than a bit of disgust.

Flickr/Thomas Hawk

As I've noted before, context matching matters in advertising . If you're looking to sell people something that highlights their individuality, then doing so in a mating context works better than in a context of fear (as animals aren't exactly aiming to look distinct when predators are nearby). The same seems to hold for using sex. While it might be useful for getting eyes on your advertisement, sex is by no means guaranteed to ensure that people like what they see once you have their attention.

In that regard, sex—like any other advertising tool—needs to be used selectively, targeting the correct audience in the correct context if it's going to succeed at increasing people's interest in buying. Sex in general doesn't sell. However, it might prove more effective for those with more promiscuous attitudes than those with more monogamous ones; it might prove useful if advertising a product related to sex or mating, but not useful for selling domain names ( like the old GoDaddy commercials ); it might work better if you associate your product with things that lead to sex (like status), rather than sex itself. These are all avenues worth pursuing further to see when, where, and why sex works or fails.

That said, it is still possible that sex might prove useful, even in some inappropriate contexts. Consider the following hypothetical example: people will consider buying a product only after they have seen an advertisement for it. Advertisement X isn't sexual, but when paired with the product will increase people's intentions to buy it by 10 percent. However, it will also not really get noticed by many people, as the content is bland. By contrast, advertisement Y is sexual, will decrease people's intentions to buy a product by 10 percent, but will also get four-times as many eyes on it. The latter ad might well be more successful, as it will capture the eye of more potential customers that may still buy the product despite the inappropriate use of sex. While targeting advertisements might be more effective, the attention model of advertising shouldn't be ruled out entirely, especially if targeting advertising would prove too cumbersome.

Wirtz, J., Sparks, J., & Zimbres, T. (2017). The effect of exposure to sexual appeals in advertisements on memory, attitude, and purchase intention: A meta-analytic review. International Journal of Advertising, https://doi.org/10.1080/02650487.2017.1334996

Jesse Marczyk Ph.D.

Jesse Marczyk, Ph.D. , studies evolutionary psychology and writes the blog Pop Psychology.

  • Find a Therapist
  • Find a Treatment Center
  • Find a Psychiatrist
  • Find a Support Group
  • Find Online Therapy
  • United States
  • Brooklyn, NY
  • Chicago, IL
  • Houston, TX
  • Los Angeles, CA
  • New York, NY
  • Portland, OR
  • San Diego, CA
  • San Francisco, CA
  • Seattle, WA
  • Washington, DC
  • Asperger's
  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Chronic Pain
  • Eating Disorders
  • Passive Aggression
  • Personality
  • Goal Setting
  • Positive Psychology
  • Stopping Smoking
  • Low Sexual Desire
  • Relationships
  • Child Development
  • Self Tests NEW
  • Therapy Center
  • Diagnosis Dictionary
  • Types of Therapy

July 2024 magazine cover

Sticking up for yourself is no easy task. But there are concrete skills you can use to hone your assertiveness and advocate for yourself.

  • Emotional Intelligence
  • Gaslighting
  • Affective Forecasting
  • Neuroscience

Read the full essay 1002 words

Character Count 4924
Word Count 1002
Sentence Count 77
Page Count 5
Characters Per Word 4.91
Words Per Sentence 13.01
9 (8th grade)
6.84 (7th grade)
69.18 (8th grade)
9.8 (9th grade)

1

majortests.com

Sex Sells Essay

SEX SELLS!!! The Naked Truth! Baring It All! An Australian academic named Dr Murray Phillips who conducted a significant report into the sexualisation of women in sport, once wrote: "It excludes many female athletes who do not fit into the appropriate body types, it glorifies certain female shapes and sends messages about what is appropriate and inappropriate for aspiring female athletes. These images fit neatly into stereotypes that have historically prevented women's sport from being accepted on par with men's sport." n that short period of time, each Olympian needs to capitalize on media exposure and endorsements to fund the next four years of training. For women this has traditionally meant playing up sex appeal. The rapid increase in popularity of female athletics has affected the entire society in a variety of ways. On one hand it is wonderful that little girls are growing up with the opportunity to be involved in athletics, but on the other hand there are new added pressures and stereotypes that are applied to female athletes that must be faced on a daily basis. As female athletics increases in recognition, large agencies have begun marketing female athletes in a more provocative manner. Some advertisement firms believe that any media attention a female athlete can get will benefit the sport overall. Others have different views regarding how a female should market herself and the sport. Sexuality is playing a larger role in society and has bled into many other facets of life including sports. The question of whether it is morally right for a female athlete to remove her clothes for a photo shoot has caused a division in the sporting arena, the marketing field, and the advertisement industry. Sexploitation in context with women's athletics is defined as types of marketing, promotion or attempts to gain media coverage which highlights the sexual attributes of female athletes, especially the visibility of their bodies. Many people believe that this has a negative affect on women and athletics because it does not send a valuable message about athletics and highlights a woman's body rather than her athletic abilities. Dr. Murray Phillips, an author who wrote a book on media coverage and women's sports, argues that there are several flaws in sexploitation. In his book he states, "It excludes many female athletes who do not fit into the appropriate body types, it glorifies certain female shapes and sends messages about what is appropriate and inappropriate for aspiring female athletes. These images fit neatly into stereotypes that have historically prevented women's sport from being accepted on par with men's sport." A basic adage of advertising involves a simple concept “sex sells.” Everyone knows and understands this to be true. Images of sex appeal are commonly used to sell a wide variety of products. Commercialized sports are products to be consumed by the mass audience. The sports mass media often utilize the “sex sells” mentality in their approach to promoting and selling sports. As a result, a great deal of objectification of athletes occur in sports. This marketing tool is most evident in the representation of young females athletes in sport today. The continued use of sex appeal by sports advertising/marketing agencies portrays a negative representation of young females in modern sport. The public are constantly smothered with the overwhelming use of images in media of sex appeal. This marketing tool is commonly utilized by agencies to sell a variety of different products. Sex sells! In todays society, we worship our sporting heroes like gods, but nobody is perfect. The media amplifies the positive and desirable qualities of an athlete and shadows the negative traits resulting in a misleading representation of athletes to the public. This is a very powerful and effective marketing tool known as sex appeal and is used by many agencies to sell sports product and merchandise. This

Related Documents: Sex Sells Essay

sex sells essay

Sex sells, as media finally delves into Libya scandal Rather than focus on the ongoing war and other issues plaguing the middle east and ergo the united states the media continues to put a shallow mask on as it once again favors sexual scandal over legitimate and paramount news. This in particular focus’ on the recent CIA Scandal in which the director of the CIA was caught having an affair with his biographer. This is an excellent illustration of how the media in modern society is focused heavily…

Words 402 - Pages 2

sex sells essay

The Uses Of Advertisers Use Sex To Sell Their Products

It’s no secret al all that advertisers use sex to sell their products. This catchy marketing strategy has been popular since advertising first emerged, and is showing no signs of slowing down. Actually, according to Jean Kilbourne in her killing us softly video, “Media Representations of women have gotten worse over the decades”. Recently, more and more companies have begun using this strategy and now most ads incorporate some form of sexual content to promote their product. The type of sexual reference…

Words 890 - Pages 4

Sex in Advertising Essay

English 12- Period 2 31 October 2014 Let’s Talk About Sex Sex. A subject usually reserved for behind closed doors that has made its way into our homes and the minds of customers everywhere through suggestive advertisements. Sexual connotations in advertising help companies to sell their products by appealing to a customer’s subconscious and making the customers want the product unknowingly because they think it will bring them sex. Advertising in itself is everywhere: in magazines, on your TV,…

Words 1511 - Pages 7

sex sells essay

Essay about Axe Attack

magazine advertisement promoting their newest product Axe Anarchy for Him and for Her. This advertisement is very effective by using an eye catching design, appealing to both sexes and the common person, and including the element of sex appeal and excitement to sell the product. There are many ads seen while flipping through an average magazine. This particular advertisement is very powerful because it grabs the attention of the audience immediately. The ad is eye catching by adding the bright…

Words 1249 - Pages 5

Cant buy me love Essay

establishments of business where sex workers provide sexual services to their patrons for pay. The top courts in Ontario recently legalized brothels saying “Canadian prostitution laws unfairly discriminate against prostitutes and their ability to work in safe environments” (Paula Newton, CNN) but even with the legalization of brothels it is still illegal to solicit costumers outside in the street. To sum up the new law legalizing brothels, it is perfectly legal to sell sex in-doors inside an established…

Words 1850 - Pages 8

Condoms: Human Sexual Behavior and Sex Essays

July 2014 The Promotion of Safe Sex is Beneficial “About half of teens 15-19yo in the U.S. have had sex at least once” (TeenHealthFX). Underage sex is seen by society as indecent and immoral, but it is extremely common among teens. Innocence is withering away at younger ages as time goes on. Sex among teens has become more prevalent because of the glamorous ways the media portrays sex and the accepting of social views on the topic. “Most young people have had sex for the first time by the age of…

Words 1975 - Pages 8

Examples Of Sex Appeal Advertising

Sex Appeal Advertising In the business world, it is important to get information to customers as quickly as possible concerning sales, inventory and price changes as well as new products or services with in the business. Advertising creates such an instant awareness and is the tool most businesses use to promote their products or services. Prosperous advertising will promote customers loyalty, build a brand by defining the companies place in the market, and separate the company from the competition…

Words 573 - Pages 3

Essay on Sex Advertising

Sex Advertising and how women are portrayed in perfume advertising. Name: Institution: Introduction Sex advertising refers to the use of sex appeal in trying to sell your products. Erotic imageries are more attractive to the eye and they get attention from us human beings, the images displayed are such as cheese cake, nudity or at times even beef cake (Taflinger, 1996). This mode of advertising began long ago where beautiful carvings of women bodies were made. Sex advertising…

Words 2268 - Pages 10

Ambrose 1 Advertising Essay

commercials with sexual appeal to trick people into buying products every day. In this modern day world advertisements use sex and an ideal person to capture the attention of the audience. Advertisements were created to make us think that we have to look a certain way for us to feel good about ourselves. “Advertising Fifteen Basic Appeals” by Jib Fowles pointed out that the need for sex and attention are the two top basic appeals in which television watchers don’t realize they are being deluded by the…

Words 920 - Pages 4

Prostitution Research Paper

Sex Sells. The word “sex” can make some people happy, some people crack up laughing, and others turn red with embarrassment. We live in a society where sex is the “thing,” we see it on TV, on billboards, in fashion magazines, and on cd covers. Why is that? Simple answer, sex sells. We all came to live with it and may not even think twice about it when it is presented right in front us. However, when it comes to the branch of prostitution, on the darker side of the sex industry, for the most part…

Words 1173 - Pages 5

ManyEssays.com

  • 1-888-302-2840
  • 1-888-422-8036
  • Sex Sells 3
  • Your research paper is written by certified writers
  • Your requirements and targets are always met
  • You are able to control the progress of your writing assignment
  • You get a chance to become an excellent student!

sex sells essay

Essay Details:

sex sells essay

Essay text:

The media dominates how young men and women should look. The popular teen magazine ?Dolly' is marketed at ages from 10 to about 16. In the current May issue it gives girls advice about "how to get better skin tomorrow" and offers detailed information about "sex ed." The pages of Dolly magazine and others alike are filled with comments aimed at young girls whether it's to sell the latest product or convince them that if they behaved in a certain way they would be happier and have more friends. Young girls are constantly reading pages filled with re- occurring beauty tips on how to look like celebrities and achieve their desirable looks...

sex sells essay

Calculate a fair price for your order

sex sells essay

Do you need an essay?

A professional team of writers is able to craft custom essays from scratch according to your instructions. We are ready to satisfy writing needs of every demanding customer.

sex sells essay

Do you need many essays?

The product provided is intended to be used for research or study purposes. Get instant access to over 200,000 papers.

Common topics in this essay:

  • HOW TO WIN FRIENDS AND INFLUENCE PEOPLE
  • How To Win Friends And Influence People
  • How to Win Friends and Influence People
  • how to win friends and influence people
  • Celebrity endorsements of high involvement brands evoke cognitive evaluations that can erode brand equity and affect celebrity credibility
  • From Early Adopters to Limited Edition to Personalised Products; People are searching for esteem needs
  • Role Of E-Recruitment To Large Companies And Advantages To Employer
  • The Pinocchio Factor In Consumer Attitudes Towards Celebrity Endorsement: Celebrity Endorsement, The Reebok Brand, And An Examination Of A Recent Campaign
  • How to win friends and influence people
  • People Influence Me
  • Rock And Rap Does Not Have A Bad Influence On Teenagers
  • A company has announced that it wishes to build a large factory near your community. Discuss the advantages and disadvantages of this new influence on your community. Do you support or oppose the factory? Explain your position.
  • Family And Religion : Influence On People
  • Some people think that governments should spend large amounts of money to explore outer space. Others think that this money should be used to improve life on earth
  • Tobacco Companies Targeting Young People

Feedback of people who used our services.

My experience with ManyEssays.com is extremely satisfying! I was amazed on your user-friendly website which is very helpful. I have also happy on how your customer service experts ...

I would like to say thank you for the level of excellence on providing written works. My University required us a very difficult paper using a very specific writing format and ...

I am happy with the results your company gives. ManyEssays.com is the best place for essays!

I was given by my professor a very difficult essay assignment and I really don’t know what to do. I needed help and ManyEssays.com came at the right time. I quickly availed your ...

I am very happy on the excellent job your writers did on my thesis. It was beautiful in every way, it was a literary masterpiece! Everything was done according to instructions and ...

A top-notch organization all the way and a model in excellent service, your company is. The level of expertise in your field is exceptional as you have in your employment the best ...

Your writing service is so amazing! I was skeptic at first on how your company provides result, but my skepticism gradually vanished immediately after you had finished one task in ...

Your services were an important factor for my academic advance during my college years. I really thank you that you were there when I needed help in my term paper. Your company ...

Similar Essays:

1 pages / 273 words

1 pages / 131 words

3 pages / 647 words

2 pages / 576 words

2 pages / 573 words

1 pages / 251 words

2 pages / 311 words

IMAGES

  1. Family Nudists possibly the FULL SET Porn Pictures, XXX Photos, Sex

    sex sells essay

  2. Mom Fucks your Friend SPH Cuckold

    sex sells essay

  3. I Spent 7 Days Learning To Love Being Intimate With My Husband Again

    sex sells essay

  4. crow bone, nicole watterson, ass, cartoon porn, furry porn, huge ass

    sex sells essay

  5. Surprise Lesbian Threesome Sex

    sex sells essay

  6. Sell Young GF

    sex sells essay

VIDEO

  1. The DARK SECRET hidden in Lovejoy's BEST song

  2. sex sells (and unfortunately i'm buying) -piebald @ bamboozle NJ 5/2/2010

  3. The Meffs

  4. Suspicious Person #3-6 Prank

  5. W H Auden

  6. Sex sells! And other mythologies of advertising

COMMENTS

  1. Essay on Sex Sells in Advertising

    768 Words. 4 Pages. Open Document. A Great Moment In Creativity. In 2010 Diesel released their spring campaign tagged, "Sex Sells, Unfortunately We Sell Jeans.". This was a follow up on their "Be Stupid" campaign, which helped re-establish the brand amongst consumers. The brands image was able to portray a sexy idea while still having ...

  2. Sex in advertising

    The book is a visual essay about sex roles in advertising and the symbolism implied in the depictions of men and women in advertising. ... This research has led to the popular idea that "sex sells". [26] Marketing strategies centered around sex have been successful. Abercrombie & Fitch used sex to market their brand in a variety of ways, ...

  3. Sex Sells Essay

    Sex Sells Essay - Free download as PDF File (.pdf), Text File (.txt) or read online for free. Scribd is the world's largest social reading and publishing site. ...

  4. Sex sells

    Advertising: Sex Sells Essay example. Magazines play a part in creating the image of sex sells. Advertisers use an image to create an advertisement to sell a product. The Purpose of advertising is not to sell sex, but to attract the consumer. Their talent is the ability to transform seemingly neutral object to create a desirable product.

  5. Does Sex Really Sell? Paradoxical Effects of Sexualization in

    To test the "sex sells" assumption, we examined how Italian men and women react to sexualized advertising. Women showed lower product attractiveness and purchase intentions toward products presented with sexualized female models than with neutral ads, whereas men were unaffected by ads' sexualization (Study 1, n = 251). Study 2 (n = 197) replicated the overall results. Study 3 (n = 198 ...

  6. Objectifying Women In Ads "Sex Sells" (pdf)

    The history of objectifying women in order to sell a product can be traced back to the early 20th century. Today, this objectification of women has created a "sexualization of culture". Non-sexual products like food, drinks, cars, and in the case of my analysis, flowers, are sexualized and convey women merely as objects.

  7. Advertising: Sex Sells Essay examples

    Advertising: Sex Sells Essay examples. Good Essays. 1365 Words. 6 Pages. Open Document. Advertising is a billion dollar market with a sole purpose to persuade the consumer to purchase some type of product or service. Companies use many different methods to convince the public to spend money on their products with most of their advertising ...

  8. Advertising: Sex Sells Essay example

    Advertising: Sex Sells Essay examples Advertising is a billion dollar market with a sole purpose to persuade the consumer to purchase some type of product or service. Companies use many different methods to convince the public to spend money on their products with most of their advertising focused around the idea of "sex sells".

  9. Sex Sells Advertisement Analysis

    Sex Sells Advertisement Analysis. 1100 Words5 Pages. "Sex sells". Human beings have priorities; food is one, and sex is another. During the entire history of advertising, brands have used sexualization and objectification in their ads, which could lead to self-esteem issues and eating disorders. As said before, sex is one of the human's ...

  10. Sex Sells

    Sex Sells. For the upcoming TED Talk, I will investigate the "Sex Sells" concept and how it relates to the recent shift in our acceptance of profanities and sexual material. There has been a dramatic increase in the abundance of obscenities in the media (television, music, films, etc.) and our tolerance of such ideas and images has ...

  11. Does Sex Sell?

    Does Sex Sell? Decent Essays. 986 Words. 4 Pages. 1 Works Cited. Open Document. When you go to Epifanio de los Santos Avenue (EDSA), we are bombarded with huge billboards in hope to catch the attention of passengers, which include pictures of people in lingerie or underwear. A few months ago, there was a controversy surrounding the Philippine ...

  12. Essay On Sex In Advertising

    Essay On Sex In Advertising. 888 Words4 Pages. You might have heard this phrase yourself once. Sex sells. It has been debated consistently and people are left wondering why oversexualized and often sexist ads still exist and are being produced. In the world of advertising, the idea of presenting sex in advertisements to sell more is one of the ...

  13. The Ethics Of Sex Appeal In Advertising Media Essay

    The issue of whether sex sells could be countered by screening the massive quantities of billboards and classified ads which integrate some form of sex appeal or nudity (Pynor, 2004, p. 42). Several reasons have been provided with the use of sexually oriented themes, among them are to achieve product and brand attention, recognition, recall ...

  14. Does Sex Actually Sell?

    Advertising: Sex Sells Essay examples Advertising is a billion dollar market with a sole purpose to persuade the consumer to purchase some type of product or service. Companies use many different methods to convince the public to spend money on their products with most of their advertising focused around the idea of "sex sells".

  15. Sex Sells. It's True Now and It Was True 100 Years Ago

    It's True Now and It Was True 100 Years Ago. - The New York Times. Sex Sells. It's True Now and It Was True 100 Years Ago. "It was not easy for the British public to 'get' O. Henry at ...

  16. Sex Sells, so Why Not Be Sold? Essay

    Additionally, every state (except some parts of Nevada) in the U.S. criminalizes women who sell sex, but only half of those states also have laws criminalizing men who buy sex (Mathieson 374). This discriminatory enforcement of prostitution laws undermines the gender equality this country is trying to achieve.

  17. Understanding Sex in Advertising

    In terms of capturing attention, sex works. Of the 20 effects looking at the recall for ads, the average size was d = .38. Interesting, this effect was slightly larger for the congruent ads (d ...

  18. Sex sells

    Sex sells Should Sex Be Sold? ... David Leonhardt and Kathleen Kerwin state in their essay, "Hey Kids, Buy This!" that, "Combining allowance, earnings, and gifts, kids 14 and under will directly spend an estimated $20 billion this year, and they will influence another $200 billion" (83). Ad designers perceive a need to shock, stand out ...

  19. Sex Sells Essay

    Sex Sells Essay. Sex sells, as media finally delves into Libya scandal Rather than focus on the ongoing war and other issues plaguing the middle east and ergo the united states the media continues to put a shallow mask on as it once again favors sexual scandal over legitimate and paramount news.

  20. Free Essays on Sex Sells

    Free Essays on Sex Sells . Search. Sex Sells. Group 1 Ana Morrobel Prof. Sodaro SOC 100-121 Gender and Sexuality/ Sex in advertising Sex in advertising is used to draw attention to customers in order to sell. In this commercial it shows Kim Kardashian as a symbol of what "you can be" if you wear Sketchers, Shape Ups. Apparently Sketchers Shape...

  21. Sex sells Essay

    Sex sells and other kinds of academic papers in our essays database at Many Essays. 1-888-302-2840; 1-888-422-8036; Home; Services. Annotated Bibliography. Article Critique. Article Review. Article Writing. Blog Article. Book Report. Book Review. Business Plan.

  22. How Sex Sells Essay

    How Sex Sells and other kinds of academic papers in our essays database at Many Essays. 1-888-302-2840; 1-888-422-8036; ×. Home ...

  23. Sex Sells Essay

    Sex Sells and other kinds of academic papers in our essays database at Many Essays. 1-888-302-2840; 1-888-422-8036;