U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • My Bibliography
  • Collections
  • Citation manager

Save citation to file

Email citation, add to collections.

  • Create a new collection
  • Add to an existing collection

Add to My Bibliography

Your saved search, create a file for external citation management software, your rss feed.

  • Search in PubMed
  • Search in NLM Catalog
  • Add to Search

Academic interventions for academic procrastination: A review of the literature

Affiliation.

  • 1 a Department of Psychology , Tel-Hai Academic College , Upper Galilee , Israel.
  • PMID: 29485384
  • DOI: 10.1080/10852352.2016.1198154

Procrastination is a widespread phenomenon in academic settings. It has been studied from many different theoretical angles, and a variety of causes and consequences have been suggested. Recent studies support the notion that academic procrastination can be seen from a situational perspective and as a failure in learning self-regulation. It suggests that interventions should address situational as well as deficits in self-regulation to help students overcome their procrastinating tendencies. The present review examined the recent literature on causes and consequences of academic procrastination and the limited number of studies of academic interventions for academic procrastination. Findings of this review strengthen the need to further study the topic of academic interventions for academic procrastination and to develop effective interventions. At the end of this review, several suggestions for the development of academic interventions are outlined.

Keywords: Academic interventions; academic procrastination; young college students.

PubMed Disclaimer

Similar articles

  • "I'll stop procrastinating now!" Fostering specific processes of self-regulated learning to reduce academic procrastination. Grunschel C, Patrzek J, Klingsieck KB, Fries S. Grunschel C, et al. J Prev Interv Community. 2018 Apr-Jun;46(2):143-157. doi: 10.1080/10852352.2016.1198166. J Prev Interv Community. 2018. PMID: 29485385
  • Academic procrastination and academic performance: An initial basis for intervention. Goroshit M. Goroshit M. J Prev Interv Community. 2018 Apr-Jun;46(2):131-142. doi: 10.1080/10852352.2016.1198157. J Prev Interv Community. 2018. PMID: 29485387
  • Is procrastination all that "bad"? A qualitative study of academic procrastination and self-worth in postgraduate university students. Abramowski A. Abramowski A. J Prev Interv Community. 2018 Apr-Jun;46(2):158-170. doi: 10.1080/10852352.2016.1198168. J Prev Interv Community. 2018. PMID: 29485386
  • Bibliometric Analysis and Visualization of Academic Procrastination. Tao X, Hanif H, Ahmed HH, Ebrahim NA. Tao X, et al. Front Psychol. 2021 Oct 18;12:722332. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.722332. eCollection 2021. Front Psychol. 2021. PMID: 34733204 Free PMC article. Review.
  • Procrastination during the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Scoping Review. Unda-López A, Osejo-Taco G, Vinueza-Cabezas A, Paz C, Hidalgo-Andrade P. Unda-López A, et al. Behav Sci (Basel). 2022 Feb 6;12(2):38. doi: 10.3390/bs12020038. Behav Sci (Basel). 2022. PMID: 35200289 Free PMC article. Review.
  • Explaining the changes in procrastination in an ACT-based course - psychological flexibility and time and effort management as mediators. Asikainen H, Hailikari T, Katajavuori N. Asikainen H, et al. Front Psychol. 2024 May 1;15:1331205. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1331205. eCollection 2024. Front Psychol. 2024. PMID: 38751759 Free PMC article.
  • More sense of self-discipline, less procrastination: the mediation of autonomous motivation. Tao S, Jing Y. Tao S, et al. Front Psychol. 2023 Nov 23;14:1268614. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1268614. eCollection 2023. Front Psychol. 2023. PMID: 38078243 Free PMC article.
  • The influence of learning styles on academic procrastination among students in mathematics. Wan Hussin WAS, Mohd Matore MEE. Wan Hussin WAS, et al. Front Psychol. 2023 Oct 26;14:1239933. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1239933. eCollection 2023. Front Psychol. 2023. PMID: 37954184 Free PMC article.
  • When procrastination pays off: Role of knowledge sharing ability, autonomous motivation, and task involvement for employee creativity. Adeel A, Sarminah S, Jie L, Kee DMH, Qasim Daghriri Y, Alghafes RA. Adeel A, et al. Heliyon. 2023 Sep 11;9(10):e19398. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2023.e19398. eCollection 2023 Oct. Heliyon. 2023. PMID: 37767479 Free PMC article.
  • Evaluating the effectiveness of a brief digital procrastination intervention targeting university students in Sweden: study protocol for the Focus randomised controlled trial. Åsberg K, Bendtsen M. Åsberg K, et al. BMJ Open. 2023 Jul 21;13(7):e072506. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2023-072506. BMJ Open. 2023. PMID: 37479522 Free PMC article.

Publication types

  • Search in MeSH

LinkOut - more resources

Full text sources.

  • Taylor & Francis

Other Literature Sources

  • scite Smart Citations
  • MedlinePlus Health Information

full text provider logo

  • Citation Manager

NCBI Literature Resources

MeSH PMC Bookshelf Disclaimer

The PubMed wordmark and PubMed logo are registered trademarks of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (HHS). Unauthorized use of these marks is strictly prohibited.

  • DOI: 10.1080/10852352.2016.1198154
  • Corpus ID: 3558145

Academic interventions for academic procrastination: A review of the literature

  • S. Zacks , Meirav Hen
  • Published in Journal of Prevention… 27 February 2018
  • Education, Psychology

90 Citations

And interventions.

  • Highly Influenced

A Systematic Review of Interventions to Reduce Academic Procrastination and Implications for Instructor-based Classroom Interventions

Academic procrastination in children and adolescents: a scoping review, change of academic procrastination by demographic characteristics: a research on university students, toward a holistic approach to reducing academic procrastination with classroom interventions, academic procrastination in the context of meaning structures of personality: why is it important for the formation of sustainable behavior, psychocentrum review, examining procrastination among university students through the lens of the self-regulated learning model, academic procrastination and academic performance: do learning disabilities matter, psychoeducation of academic procrastination in undergraduate students, 58 references, why students procrastinate: a qualitative approach, exploring reasons and consequences of academic procrastination: an interview study.

  • Highly Influential

An investigation of the efficacy of acceptance-based behavioral therapy for academic procrastination.

Academic procrastination, emotional intelligence, academic self-efficacy, and gpa, the relationship between procrastination and academic performance: a meta-analysis, reducing academic procrastination through a group treatment program: a pilot study, academic procrastination in college students: the role of self-reported executive function, comparing active delay and procrastination from a self-regulated learning perspective, why not procrastinate development and validation of a new active procrastination scale, academic procrastinators , strategic delayers and something betwixt and between : an interview study, related papers.

Showing 1 through 3 of 0 Related Papers

Information

  • Author Services

Initiatives

You are accessing a machine-readable page. In order to be human-readable, please install an RSS reader.

All articles published by MDPI are made immediately available worldwide under an open access license. No special permission is required to reuse all or part of the article published by MDPI, including figures and tables. For articles published under an open access Creative Common CC BY license, any part of the article may be reused without permission provided that the original article is clearly cited. For more information, please refer to https://www.mdpi.com/openaccess .

Feature papers represent the most advanced research with significant potential for high impact in the field. A Feature Paper should be a substantial original Article that involves several techniques or approaches, provides an outlook for future research directions and describes possible research applications.

Feature papers are submitted upon individual invitation or recommendation by the scientific editors and must receive positive feedback from the reviewers.

Editor’s Choice articles are based on recommendations by the scientific editors of MDPI journals from around the world. Editors select a small number of articles recently published in the journal that they believe will be particularly interesting to readers, or important in the respective research area. The aim is to provide a snapshot of some of the most exciting work published in the various research areas of the journal.

Original Submission Date Received: .

  • Active Journals
  • Find a Journal
  • Proceedings Series
  • For Authors
  • For Reviewers
  • For Editors
  • For Librarians
  • For Publishers
  • For Societies
  • For Conference Organizers
  • Open Access Policy
  • Institutional Open Access Program
  • Special Issues Guidelines
  • Editorial Process
  • Research and Publication Ethics
  • Article Processing Charges
  • Testimonials
  • Preprints.org
  • SciProfiles
  • Encyclopedia

children-logo

Article Menu

academic procrastination literature review

  • Subscribe SciFeed
  • PubMed/Medline
  • Google Scholar
  • on Google Scholar
  • Table of Contents

Find support for a specific problem in the support section of our website.

Please let us know what you think of our products and services.

Visit our dedicated information section to learn more about MDPI.

JSmol Viewer

Academic procrastination in children and adolescents: a scoping review.

academic procrastination literature review

1. Introduction

2.1. inclusion and exclusion criteria, 2.1.1. documents, 2.1.2. concept, 2.1.3. context, 2.1.4. studies, 2.1.5. participants, 2.1.6. evaluation, 2.2. search strategy, 2.3. sources of information, 2.3.1. formal strategies, 2.3.2. informal strategies, 2.3.3. retrospective strategies, 2.4. coding and identification of records and data extraction, 3.1. production and evolution of publications, 3.2. characteristics of the studies, 3.2.1. participants, 3.2.2. instruments for assessing academic procrastination, 3.2.3. methodology of the studies and type of design, 3.3. content analysis, 3.3.1. investigated correlates, 3.3.2. types of interventions, 4. discussion, 5. conclusions.

  • Addressing academic procrastination in children and adolescents should consider both individual and contextual factors, as well as appropriate interventions;
  • There is a need for the development of more appropriate assessment tools to measure academic procrastination in children and adolescents, considering their specific developmental characteristics;
  • The prevalence of academic procrastination in this population is still understudied, highlighting a research gap that requires further attention;
  • In summary, further research and interventions are necessary to improve the understanding and management of academic procrastination in children and adolescents.

Author Contributions

Institutional review board statement, informed consent statement, data availability statement, conflicts of interest.

SectionItemPRISMA-ScR Checklist ItemReported on Page # (Number)
Title
Title1Identify the report as a scoping review.1
Abstract
Structured summary2Provide a structured summary including, as applicable: Background, objectives,
eligibility criteria, sources of evidence, charting methods, results, and conclusions that relate to the review question(s) and objective(s).
1
Introduction
Rationale3Describe the rationale for the review in the context of what is already known. Explain why the review question(s)/objective(s) lend themselves to a scoping review approach.1–2
Objectives4Provide an explicit statement of the question(s) and objective(s) being addressed with reference to their key elements (e.g., population or participants, concepts, and context), or other relevant key elements used to conceptualize the review question(s) and/or objective(s)).2
Methods
Protocol, and registration5Indicate if a review protocol exists, if and where it can be accessed (e.g., web
address), and, if available, provide registration information including registration number.
3
Eligibility
criteria
6Specify the characteristics of the sources of evidence (e.g., years considered,
language, publication status) used as criteria for eligibility, and provide a rationale.
3, 6
Information sources7Describe all information sources (e.g., databases with dates of coverage, contact with authors to identify additional sources) in the search, as well as the date the most recent search was executed.4
Search8Present the full electronic search strategy for at least one database, including any limits used, in order that it could be repeated.4
Selection of sources of evidence9State the process for selecting sources of evidence (i.e., screening, eligibility) included.5
Data charting process10Describe the methods of charting data from the included sources of evidence (e.g., piloted forms, forms that have been tested by the team before their use, whether data charting was carried out independently, in duplicate) and any processes for obtaining and confirming data from investigators.5
Data items11List and define all variables for which data were sought and any assumptions and simplifications made.5
Critical appraisal of individual sources of
evidence
12If performed, provide a rationale for conducting a critical appraisal of included sources of evidence, describe the methods used, and how this information was used in any data
synthesis (if appropriate).
N/A
Summary
measures
13Not applicable for scoping reviews.
Synthesis of
results
14Describe the methods of handling and summarizing the data that were charted.5
Risk of bias
across studies
15Not applicable for scoping reviews.
Additional analyses16Not applicable for scoping reviews.
Results
Selection of sources of
evidence
17Give numbers of sources of evidence screened, assessed for eligibility, and included in the review, with reasons for exclusions at each stage, ideally using a flow diagram.6
Characteristics of
sources of evidence
18For each source of evidence, present characteristics for which data were charted and provide the citations.6, 7, 8, 10
Critical appraisal within sources of
evidence
19If performed, present data on critical appraisal of included sources of evidence (see item 12).
Results of individual
sources of evidence
20For each included source of evidence, present the relevant data that were charted that relate to the review question(s) and objective(s).
Synthesis of results21Summarize and/or present the charting results as they relate to the review question(s) and objective(s).5, 6, 7, 8, 9
Risk of bias across studies22Not applicable for scoping reviews.
Additional analyses23Not applicable for scoping reviews.
Discussion
Summary of evidence24Summarize the main results (including an overview of concepts, themes, and types of evidence available), explain how they relate to the review question(s) and objectives, and consider the relevance to key groups.11–12
Limitations25Discuss the limitations of the scoping review process.13
Conclusions26Provide a general interpretation of the results with respect to the review question(s) and objective(s), as well as potential implications and/or next steps.13
Funding
Funding27Describe sources of funding for the included sources of evidence, as well as sources of funding for the scoping review. Describe the role of the funders of the scoping review.14
Bibliometric InformationSampleDesign
AuthorYearDoc. TypeEdu LevelNCountry
Milgram et al. [ ]1995ArticleSS195ISREPF
Owens and Newbegin [ ]1997ArticlePS418AUSEPF
Milgram and Toubiana [ ]1999ArticleSS245ISREPF
Owens and Newbegin [ ]2000ArticleSS380AUSEPF
Davis [ ]2001ThesisSS284USAEPF
Nadeau et al. [ ]2003ArticleSS100CANEPF
Jaradat [ ]2004ThesisSS729JORRCT
Dietz and et al. [ ]2007ArticleSS704DEUEPF
Rosário et al. [ ]2008ArticleSS533/796PRTEPF
Klassen and Kuzucu [ ]2009ArticleSS508TUREPF
Klassen et al. [ ]2009ArticleSS612CAN, SGPEPF
Uzun Özer [ ]2009ArticleSS223TUREPF
Rosário et al. [ ]2009ArticleSS580/809PRTEPF
Al-Attiyah [ ]2010ArticlePS538QATEPF
Kalafat et al. [ ]2010ArticleSS285TUREPF
Kuhnle et al. [ ]2011ArticleSS348DEUEPF
Uzun Özer and Ferrari [ ]2011ArticleSS214TUREPF
Liu and Lu [ ]2011ArticleSS712CHNEPF
Hofer et al. [ ]2012ArticleSS697DEUEPF
Motie et al. [ ]2012ArticleSS250IRNEPF
Mih [ ]2013ArticleSS189ROUEPF
Motie et al. [ ]2013ArticleSS66IRNRCT
Bong et al. [ ]2014ArticleSS306KOREPF
Katz et al. [ ]2014ArticlePS171ISREPF
Tang et al. [ ]2014ArticleSS460CHNEPF
Hong et al. [ ]2015ArticleSS597TWNEPF
Jahromi et al. [ ]2015ArticleSS268IRNEPF
Gazidari et al. [ ]2016ArticleSS311IRNEPF
Ng [ ]2016ArticleSS442SGPEPF
Shih [ ]2016ArticleSS405TWNEPF
Zohreyi et al. [ ]2016ArticleSS210IRNEPF
Chen and Han [ ]2017ArticleSS577CHNEPF
Günlü and Ceyhan [ ]2017ArticleSS1088TUREPF
Kocoglu and Emiroglu [ ]2017ArticlePS31TURRNCT
Shih [ ]2017ArticleSS405TWNEPF
Yang et al. [ ]2017ArticleSS360CHNEPF
Borekci and Uyangor [ ]2018ArticleSS496TUREPF
Jiang et al. [ ]2018ArticleSS848KOREPF
Korkmaz et al. [ ]2018ArticleSS167TUREPF
Mostafa [ ]2018ArticleSS100EGYEPF
Ziegler and Opdenakker [ ]2018ArticleSS566NLDEPF
Ghadampour and Beiranvand [ ]2019ArticleSS30IRNRCT
Macias and Nevarez [ ]2019ArticleSS300MEXEPF
Ahmadi et al. [ ]2020ArticleSS149IRNEPF
Sula Atas and Kumcagiz [ ]2020ArticleSS602TUREPF
Durak [ ]2020ArticleSS451TUREPF
Eissa and Khalifa [ ]2020ArticleSS228EGYEPF
Gündüz [ ]2020ArticleSS358TUREPF
Lenggono and Tentama [ ]2020ArticleSS60IDNEPF
Saad [ ]2020ArticleSS68EGYEPF
Santyasa et al. [ ]2020ArticleSS124IDNRNCT
Tras and Gökçen [ ]2020ArticleSS599TUREPF
Trujillo-Chumán and Noé-Grijalva [ ]2020ArticleSS366PEREPF
Zhen et al. [ ]2020ArticleSS1505CHNEPF
Çam and Ögülmüs [ ]2021ArticleSS1068TUREPF
Fulano et al. [ ]2021ArticleSS1000MOZEPF
Hong et al. [ ]2021ArticleSS633CHNEPF
Khalifa [ ]2021ArticleSS250SAUEPF
Kheirkhah [ ]2021ArticleSS559IRNRCT
Niu et al. [ ]2021ArticleSS623CHNEPF
Opdenakker [ ]2021ArticleSS687NLDEPF
Quispe et al. [ ]2021ArticleSS36PEREPF
Rezaei and Zebardast [ ]2021ArticleSS350IRNEPF
Selçuk et al. [ ]2021ArticleSS1000TUREPF
Wang [ ]2021ArticleSS566CHNEPF
Xue et al. [ ]2021ArticlePS338CHNEPF
Yang et al. [ ]2021ArticleSS353CHNEPF
Yildiz and Iskender [ ]2021ArticleSS1068TURRCT
Yu et al. [ ]2021ArticleSS24CHNEPF
Asmali and Sayin [ ]2022ArticlePS245TUREPF
Chaji and Ebrahimpour [ ]2022ArticlePS662CHNEPF
Dong and Izadpanah [ ]2022ArticleSS5IRNRCT
Galindo-Contreras and Olivas-Ugarte [ ]2022ArticleSS256PEREPF
Gezgin [ ]2022ArticleSS300TUREPF
Lubis and Djuwita [ ]2022ArticleSS1255IDNPE
Muarifah et al. [ ]2022ArticleSS429IDNEPF
Salluca et al. [ ]2022ArticleSS370PEREPF
Üztemur and Dinç [ ]2022ArticleSS136TUREPF
Zhao et al. [ ]2022ArticleSS514CHNEPF
  • Rothblum, E.D.; Solomon, L.J.; Murakami, J. Affective, Cognitive, and Behavioral Differences between High and Low Procrastinators. J. Couns. Psychol. 1986 , 33 , 387–394. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Senecal, C.; Koestner, R.; Vallerand, R.J. Self-Regulation and Academic Procrastination. J. Soc. Psychol. 1995 , 135 , 607–619. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Counseling the Procrastinator in Academic Settings ; Schouwenburg, H.C.; Lay, C.H.; Pychyl, T.A.; Ferrari, J.R. (Eds.) American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2004. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Steel, P. The Nature of Procrastination: A Meta-Analytic and Theoretical Review of Quintessential Self-Regulatory Failure. Psychol. Bull. 2007 , 133 , 65–94. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Burka, J.B.; Yuen, L.M. Procrastination: Why You Do It, What to Do about It ; Addison-Wesley: Boston, MA, USA, 1983. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Díaz-Morales, J. Procrastination: A Review of Scales and Correlates. Rev. Iberoam. Diagn. Eval. Aval. Psicol. 2019 , 51 , 43–60. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Blunt, A.K.; Pychyl, T.A. Task Aversiveness and Procrastination: A Multi-Dimensional Approach to Task Aversiveness across Stages of Personal Projects. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2000 , 28 , 153–167. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Schouwenburg, H.C. Procrastinators and Fear of Failure: An Exploration of Reasons for Procrastination. Eur. J. Personal. 1992 , 6 , 225–236. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Sirois, F.M.; Giguere, B. Giving in When Feeling Less Good: Procrastination, Action Control, and Social Temptations. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2018 , 57 , 404–427. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Klassen, R.M.; Krawchuk, L.L.; Rajani, S. Academic Procrastination of Undergraduates: Low Self-Efficacy to Self-Regulate Predicts Higher Levels of Procrastination. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2008 , 33 , 915–931. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Steel, P.; Ferrari, J.R. Sex, Education and Procrastination: An Epidemiological Study of Procrastinators’ Characteristics from a Global Sample: Sex, Education and Procrastination. Eur. J. Personal. 2013 , 27 , 51–58. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Schubert Walker, L.J.; Stewart, D.W. Overcoming the Powerlessness of Procrastination. Guid. Couns. 2000 , 16 , 39. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ferrari, J.R.; Johnson, J.L.; McCown, W.G. Procrastination and Task Avoidance: Theory, Research, and Treatment ; Springer Science & Business Media: New York, NY, USA, 1995; ISBN 978-0-306-44842-3. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Salluca, M.Y.; Valeriano, D.Y.A.; Gutierrez, R.A.; Casa-Coila, M.D. Social network addiction and academic procrastination in Peruvian adolescents in times of coronavirus COVID-19. Rev. Electron. Interuniv. Form. Profr. 2022 , 25 , 129–143. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Muarifah, A.; Rofiah, N.H.; Mujidin, M.; Mohamad, Z.S.; Oktaviani, F. Students’ Academic Procrastination during the COVID-19 Pandemic: How Does Adversity Quotient Mediate Parental Social Support? Front. Educ. 2022 , 7 , 516. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Üztemur, S.; Dinç, E. Academic Procrastination Using Social Media: A Cross-sectional Study with Middle School Students on the Buffering and Moderator Roles of Self-control and Gender. Psychol. Sch. 2022 , 60 , 1060–1082. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kim, K.R.; Seo, E.H. The Relationship between Procrastination and Academic Performance: A Meta-Analysis. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2015 , 82 , 26–33. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Huntley, C.D.; Young, B.; Temple, J.; Longworth, M.; Smith, C.T.; Jha, V.; Fisher, P.L. The Efficacy of Interventions for Test-Anxious University Students: A Meta-Analysis of Randomized Controlled Trials. J. Anxiety Disord. 2019 , 63 , 36–50. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Amani, H.; Habibi-Kaleybar, R.; Farid, A.; Sheykhalizadeh, S. Meta-Analysis of the Effectiveness of Interventions on test anxiety reduction. Psychol. Res. 2021 , 45 , 56. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Sirois, F.M. Procrastination and Stress: Exploring the Role of Self-Compassion. Self Identity 2013 , 13 , 128–145. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Sirois, F.M. Procrastination and Stress: A Conceptual Review of Why Context Matters. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023 , 20 , 5031. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Sirois, F.M.; Pychyl, T.A. Procrastination, Health, and Well-Being ; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2016. [ Google Scholar ]
  • López-González, M.A.; De-María, B.; Rubio-Garay, F.; Rodríguez-Cifuentes, F.; Fernández-Salinero San Martín, S.; Latorre, F.; Topa, G. Recuperación laboral en empleados fuera del trabajo: Una revisión paraguas de revisiones sistemáticas y meta-análisis [Recovery from Work: An Umbrella Review of Systematic Reviews and Meta-analysis]. Accion Psicol. 2022 , 19 , 1–20. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • López-González, M.A.; Morales-Landazábal, P.; Topa, G. Psychodrama Group Therapy for Social Issues: A Systematic Review of Controlled Clinical Trials. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021 , 18 , 4442. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Tricco, A.C.; Lillie, E.; Zarin, W.; O’Brien, K.K.; Colquhoun, H.; Levac, D.; Moher, D.; Peters, M.D.J.; Horsley, T.; Weeks, L.; et al. PRISMA Extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR): Checklist and Explanation. Ann. Intern. Med. 2018 , 169 , 467–473. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ] [ Green Version ]
  • Aromataris, E.; Munn, Z. JBI Manual for Evidence Synthesis ; JBI: Adelaide, Australia, 2020. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Munn, Z.; Stern, C.; Aromataris, E.; Lockwood, C.; Jordan, Z. What Kind of Systematic Review Should I Conduct? A Proposed Typology and Guidance for Systematic Reviewers in the Medical and Health Sciences. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2018 , 18 , 5. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Hidalgo-Fuentes, S.; Martinez-Alvarez, I.; Sospedra-Baeza, M.J. Self-esteem and procrastination in the academic field: A meta-analysis. Rev. Fuentes 2022 , 24 , 77–89. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Lu, D.; He, Y.; Tan, Y. Gender, Socioeconomic Status, Cultural Differences, Education, Family Size and Procrastination: A Sociodemographic Meta-Analysis. Front. Psychol. 2022 , 12 , 719425. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Pereira, L.D.C.; Ramos, F.P. Academic Procrastination in University Students. A Systematic Review of the Literature. Psicol. Esc. E Educ. 2021 , 25 . [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Zacks, S.; Hen, M. Academic Interventions for Academic Procrastination: A Review of the Literature. J. Prev. Interv. Community 2018 , 46 , 117–130. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Milgram, N.; Toubiana, Y. Academic Anxiety, Academic Procrastination, and Parental Involvement in Students and Their Parents. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 1999 , 69 , 345–361. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Owens, A.M.; Newbegin, I. Academic Procrastination of Adolescents in English, and Mathematics: Gender and Personality Variations. J. Soc. Behav. Personal. 2000 , 15 , 111–124. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Klassen, R.M.; Kuzucu, E. Academic Procrastination and Motivation of Adolescents in Turkey. Educ. Psychol. 2009 , 29 , 69–81. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Dong, Y.; Izadpanah, S. The Effect of Corrective Feedback from Female Teachers on Formative Assessments: Educational Resilience, Educational Belongingness, and Academic Procrastination in an English Language Course. Curr. Psychol. J. Divers. Perspect. Divers. Psychol. Issues 2022 . [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kheirkhah, A. Investigating the Effect of Self-Efficacy Training and Learning Strategies on Academic Procrastination of Female Students. Int. J. Pharm. Res. 2021 , 13 , 6267–6276. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Lubis, N.Y.; Djuwita, E. Penerapan Group Cognitive Behavior Therapy (CBT) Untuk Meurunkan Prokrastinasi Akademik Pada Siswi Smp Yang Menjalani Pendidikan Jarak Jauh (PJJ). J. Syntax Lit. 2022 , 7 , 1446–1460. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Mostafa, A.A. Academic Procrastination, Self-Efficacy Beliefs, and Academic Achievement among Middle School First Year Students with Learning Disabilities. Int. J. Psycho Educ. Sci. 2018 , 7 , 87–93. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zohreyi, T.; Erfani, N.; Jadidiyan, A.A. Predicting Academic Stress Base on Academic Procrastination among Female Students. Acta Med. Mediterr. 2016 , 32 , 1257–1264. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tuckman, B.W. The Development and Concurrent Validity of the Procrastination Scale. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1991 , 51 , 473–480. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Çakici, D.C. General and Academic in High School and University Students Examination of Procrastination Behavior. Unpublished. Master’s Thesis, Ankara University, Ankara, Turkey, 2003. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Aitken, M.E. A Personality Profile of the College Student Procrastinator ; University of Pittsburg: Pittsburg, PA, USA, 1982. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lay, C.H. At Last, My Research Article on Procrastination. J. Res. Personal. 1986 , 20 , 474–495. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Busko, B.; Deborah, A. Causes and Consequences of Perfectionism and Procrastination: A Structural Equation Model. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Guelph, Guelph, ON, Canada, 1998. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Huang, C.E. Self-Efficacy for Self-Regulation, Academic Anxiety, and Academic Procrastination of College Students: An Examination of Different Academic Situations and Procrastinators. Unpublished. Master’s Thesis, National Pingtung University, Pingtung, Taiwan, 2009. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Dietz, F.; Hofer, M.; Fries, S. Individual Values, Learning Routines and Academic Procrastination. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2007 , 77 , 893–906. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • McCloskey, J. Finally, My Thesis on Academic Procrastination. Master’s Thesis, University of Texas, Austin, TX, USA, 2012. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Zuo, Y. An Exploratory Design of the Questionnaire of Middle School Students’ Academic Procrastination. Unpublished. Master’s Thesis, Southwest University, Chongqing, China, 2010. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ran, H. The Preliminary Compiling of the Academic Procrastination Questionnaire. Master’s Thesis, Southwest University, Chongqing, China, 2010. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Lay, C.; Silverman, S. Trait Procrastination, Anxiety, and Dilatory Behavior. Personal. Individ. Differ. 1996 , 21 , 61–67. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Savari, K. Construction and Standarization of Academic Procrastination Test. Educ. Measurement. 2011 , 1–15. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rosário, P.; Costa, M.; Núñez, J.C.; González-Pienda, J.; Solano, P.; Valle, A. Academic Procrastination: Associations with Personal, School, and Family Variables. Span. J. Psychol. 2009 , 12 , 118–127. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Steel, P. Arousal, Avoidant and Decisional Procrastinators: Do They Exist? Personal. Individ. Differ. 2010 , 48 , 926–934. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Mann, L.; Burnett, P.; Radford, M.; Ford, S. The Melbourne Decision Making Questionnaire: An Instrument for Measuring Patterns for Coping with Decisional Conflict. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 1997 , 10 , 1–19. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Depreeuw, E.; Lens, W. Vragenlijst Aangaande Studie-Organisatievaardigheden-VaSOV [Study Management Skills: A Questionnaire] ; Catholic University Leuven, Center for Research in Motivation and Time Perspective: Leuven, Belgium, 1998. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Santyasa, I.W.; Rapi, N.K.; Sara, I.W.W. Project Based Learning and Academic Procrastination of Students in Learning Physics. Int. J. Instr. 2020 , 13 , 489–508. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Shih, S.S. An Examination of Factors Related to Taiwanese Adolescents Time Management and Academic Procrastination. Int. J. Psychol. 2016 , 51 , 513. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ocak, G.; Boyraz, S. Examination of the Relation between Academic Procrastination and Time Management Skills of Undergraduate Students in Terms of Some Variables. J. Educ. Train. Stud. 2016 , 4 , 76–84. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Günlü, A.; Ceyhan, A.A. Investigating Adolescents’ Behaviors on the Internet and Problematic Internet Usage. Addicta Turk. J. Addict. 2017 , 4 , 96–117. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Eisaa Saad, M.A. Self-Regulated Learning and Academic Procrastination as Predictors of Smartphone Addiction among Second Year-Middle School Learning Disabled Students. Rev. Amazon. Investiga 2020 , 9 , 236–243. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Quispe, G.C.; Carazas, S.M.C.; Marron, M.S.C.; Quintanilla, A.V. Academic procrastination: A descriptive study with high school students. Apunt. Univ. 2021 , 11 , 40–59. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Ziegler, N.; Opdenakker, M.-C. The Development of Academic Procrastination in First-Year Secondary Education Students: The Link with Metacognitive Self-Regulation, Self-Efficacy, and Effort Regulation. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2018 , 64 , 71–82. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Shih, S.-S. Factors Related to Taiwanese Adolescents’ Academic Procrastination, Time Management, and Perfectionism. J. Educ. Res. 2017 , 110 , 415–424. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Zhao, Y.; Elder, K.G. Evaluating Pharmacy Student Perceptions and Effectiveness of Procrastination Prevention Events. Curr. Pharm. Teach. Learn. 2020 , 12 , 570–576. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Zhen, R.; Li, L.; Ding, Y.; Hong, W.; Liu, R.-D. How Does Mobile Phone Dependency Impair Academic Engagement among Chinese Left-behind Children? Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2020 , 116 , 105169. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Xue, X.; Wang, Y.; Li, H.; Gao, J.; Si, J. The Association between Mathematical Attitudes, Academic Procrastination and Mathematical Achievement among Primary School Students: The Moderating Effect of Mathematical Metacognition. Curr. Psychol. 2021 , 42 , 7953–7964. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Owens, A.M.; Newbegin, I. Procrastination in High School Achievement: A Causal Structural Model. J. Soc. Behav. Personal. 1997 , 12 , 869–887. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Bong, M.; Hwang, A.; Noh, A.; Kim, S. Perfectionism and Motivation of Adolescents in Academic Contexts. J. Educ. Psychol. 2014 , 106 , 711–729. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Jiang, Y.; Rosenzweig, E.Q.; Gaspard, H. An Expectancy-Value-Cost Approach in Predicting Adolescent Students’ Academic Motivation and Achievement. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2018 , 54 , 139–152. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kalafat, T.; Kağan, M.; Boysan, M.; Güngör, İ. Associations between Academic Competence and Obsessive-Compulsive Symptoms among Adolescents. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 2010 , 5 , 309–313. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Katz, I.; Eilot, K.; Nevo, N. “I’ll Do It Later”: Type of Motivation, Self-Efficacy and Homework Procrastination. Motiv. Emot. 2014 , 38 , 111–119. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Tang, K.; Deng, X.; Fan, F.; Long, K.; Wang, H.; Zhang, Y. Mediating Effect of Academic Self-Efficacy between Parenting Style and Academic Procrastination. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 2014 , 22 , 889–892. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Çam, Z.; Ögülmüs, S. Testing of a Model on the School Burnout among High School Students and Exploring the Model’s Prediction Level of Grade Retention. Int. J. Curric. Instr. 2021 , 13 , 950–985. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Davis, J.K. The Effects of Culture on High School Academic Procrastination ; ProQuest Information & Learning: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2001; Volume 61. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Rosário, P.; Núñez, J.C.; Salgado, A.; González-Pienda, J.A.; Valle, A.; Joly, C.; Bernardo, A. Test Anxiety: Associations with Personal and Family Variables. Psicothema 2008 , 20 , 563–570. [ Google Scholar ] [ PubMed ]
  • Borekci, C.; Uyangor, N. Family Attitude, Academic Procrastination and Test Anxiety as Predictors of Academic Achievement. Int. J. Educ. Methodol. 2018 , 4 , 219–226. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Mih, V. Role of Parental Support for Learning, Autonomous/Control Motivation, and Forms of Self-Regulation on Academic Attainment in High School Students: A Path Analysis. Cogn. Brain Behav. 2013 , 17 , 35–59. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hong, J.C.; Hwang, M.Y.; Kuo, Y.C.; Hsu, W.Y. Parental Monitoring and Helicopter Parenting Relevant to Vocational Student’s Procrastination and Self-Regulated Learning. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2015 , 42 , 139–146. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Zhao, W.; Wang, X.; Li, J.; Li, Q.; Chen, C. “Time Is My Own Treasure”: Parental Autonomy Support and Academic Procrastination among Chinese Adolescents. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 2022 , 15 , 2773–2782. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Yıldız Durak, H. Modeling of Variables Related to Problematic Internet Usage and Problematic Social Media Usage in Adolescents. Curr. Psychol. 2020 , 39 , 1375–1387. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Hong, W.; Liu, R.-D.; Ding, Y.; Jiang, S.; Yang, X.; Sheng, X. Academic Procrastination Precedes Problematic Mobile Phone Use in Chinese Adolescents: A Longitudinal Mediation Model of Distraction Cognitions. Addict. Behav. 2021 , 121 , 106993. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Tras, Z.; Gökçen, G. Academic Procrastination and Social Anxiety as Predictive Variables Internet Addiction of Adolescents. Int. Educ. Stud. 2020 , 13 , 23–35. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Khalifa, A.G. Social Networking Addiction and Quality of Academic Life among First-Year High School Students in Saudi Arabia: The Mediating Role of Academic Procrastination. J. Intellect. Disabil. Diagn. Treat. 2021 , 9 , 374–380. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Gezgin, D.M. Gender, Self-Regulation, Academic Procrastination, and Smartphone Checking Frequency During Study Hours in Predicting Turkish Adolescents’ Smartphone Addiction. Addicta Turk. J. Addict. 2022 , 9 , 38–47. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kocoglu, D.; Emiroglu, O.N. The Impact of Comprehensive School Nursing Services on Students’ Academic Performance. J. Caring Sci. 2017 , 6 , 5–17. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ] [ Green Version ]
  • Ghadampour, E.; Beiranvand, K. Effect of Cognitive and Metacognitive Learning Strategies Training on Academic Procrastination and Self Efficacy in Students. Adv. Cogn. Sci. 2019 , 21 , 31–41. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Motie, H.; Heidari, M.; Sadeghi, M.A. Development of A Self -Regulation Package for Academic Procrastination and Evaluation of Its Effectiveness. Eur. J. Soc. Behav. Sci. 2013 , 4 , 889–896. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Jaradat, A.K.M. Test Anxiety in Jordanian Students: Measurement, Correlates and Treatment. 2004. Available online: https://archiv.ub.uni-marburg.de/diss/z2004/0124/pdf/dakmj.pdf (accessed on 22 November 2022).
  • Yildiz, B.; Iskender, M. The Secure Attachment Style Oriented Psycho-Educational Program for Reducing Intolerance of Uncertainty and Academic Procrastination: Yıldız & Iskender: Secure Attachment, Academic Procrastination & Intolerance of Uncertainty. Curr. Psychol. 2021 , 40 , 1850–1863. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Van Eerde, W. Procrastination in Academic Settings and the Big Five Model of Personality: A Meta-Analysis. In Counseling the Procrastinator in Academic Settings ; Schouwenburg, H.C., Lay, C.H., Pychyl, T.A., Ferrari, J.R., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2004; pp. 29–40. ISBN 1-59147-107-9. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Van Eerde, W. A Meta-Analytically Derived Nomological Network of Procrastination. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2003 , 35 , 1401–1418. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Van Eerde, W.; Klingsieck, K.B. Overcoming Procrastination? A Meta-Analysis of Intervention Studies. Educ. Res. Rev. 2018 , 25 , 73–85. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Sharin, A.N. E-Learning During COVID-19: A Review of Literature. J. Pengaj. Media Malays. 2021 , 23 , 15–28. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Mann, L. Cross-Cultural Differences in Self-Reported Decision-Making Style and Confidence. Int. J. Psychol. 1998 , 33 , 325–335. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Zimmerman, B.J. From Cognitive Modeling to Self-Regulation: A Social Cognitive Career Path. Educ. Psychol. 2013 , 48 , 135–147. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Zimmerman, B.J.; Schunk, D.H. Self-Regulated Learning and Performance: An Introduction and an Overview. In Handbook of Self-Regulation of Learning and Performance ; Zimmerman, B.J., Schunk, D.H., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2011; pp. 1–12. ISBN 978-0-203-83901-0. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Malkoç, A.; Mutlu, A.K. Academic Self-Efficacy and Academic Procrastination: Exploring the Mediating Role of Academic Motivation in Turkish University Students. Univers. J. Educ. Res. 2018 , 6 , 2087–2093. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Tice, D.M.; Bratslavsky, E.; Baumeister, R.F. Emotional Distress Regulation Takes Precedence over Impulse Control: If You Feel Bad, Do It! J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2001 , 80 , 53–67. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Quirin, M.; Kuhl, J. Positive Affect, Self-Access, and Health: Research Based on PSI Theory. Z. Gesundh. 2008 , 16 , 139–142. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • González-Brignardello, M.P.; Sánchez-Elvira-Paniagua, Á. Can engagement buffer the harmful effects of academic procrastination? Acción Psicol. 2013 , 10 , 117–134. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Cazan, A.-M. Learning Motivation, Engagement and Burnout among University Students. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 2015 , 187 , 413–417. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Milgram, N.; Marshevsky, S.; Sadeh, C. Correlates of Academic Procrastination: Discomfort, Task Aversiveness, and Task Capability. J. Psychol. Interdiscip. Appl. 1995 , 129 , 145–155. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Nadeau, M.F.; Senecal, C.; Guay, F. Determinants of academic procrastination: A mediation model of family context and self-system processes. Can. J. Behav. Sci.-Rev. Can. Sci. Comport. 2003 , 35 , 97–110. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Klassen, R.M.; Ang, R.P.; Chong, W.H.; Krawchuk, L.L.; Huan, V.S.; Wong, I.Y.F.; Yeo, L.S. A Cross-Cultural Study of Adolescent Procrastination. J. Res. Adolesc. 2009 , 19 , 799–811. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Uzun Özer, B. Academic Procrastination in Group of High School Students: Frequency, Possible Reasons and Role of Hope. Turk. Psychol. Couns. Guid. J. 2009 , 4 , 18–19. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Al-Attiyah, A. Academic Procrastination and Its Relation to Motivation and Self-Efficacy: The Case of Qatari Primary School Students. Int. J. Learn. 2010 , 17 , 173–186. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Kuhnle, C.; Hofer, M.; Kilian, B. The Relationship of Self-Control, Procrastination, Motivational Interference and Regret with School Grades and Life Balance. Diskurs Kindh. Jugendforsch./Discourse J. Child. Adolesc. Res. 2011 , 6 , 31–44. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Uzun Özer, B.; Ferrari, J.R. Gender Orientation and Academic Procrastination: Exploring Turkish High School Students. Individ. Differ. Res. 2011 , 9 , 33–40. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Liu, M.-Z.; Lu, G.-Z. Reliability and Validity of Aitken Procrastination Inventory in Middle School Students. Chin. Ment. Health J. 2011 , 25 , 380–384. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Hofer, M.; Kuhnle, C.; Kilian, B.; Fries, S. Cognitive Ability and Personality Variables as Predictors of School Grades and Test Scores in Adolescents. Learn. Instr. 2012 , 22 , 368–375. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Motie, H.; Heidari, M.; Sadeghi, M.A. Predicting Academic Procrastination during Self-Regulated Learning in Iranian First Grade High School Students. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 2012 , 69 , 2299–2308. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Jahromi, R.G.; Hejazi, E.; Ejei, J.; Fard, M.K. Investigating the Mediating Role of Achievement Goals in the Relationship between Need for Cognition and Cognitive Engagement: The Effect of Procrastination Context. J. Psychol. 2015 , 19 , 3–21. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Gazidari, E.; Lavasani, M.G.; Ejei, J. The Relationship between Academic Identity and Self-Regulation Learning Strategies with Academic Procrastinate Students. J. Psychol. 2016 , 19 , 346–362. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Ng, B. Towards Lifelong Learning: Identifying Learner Profiles on Procrastination and Self-Regulation. New Waves-Educ. Res. Dev. J. 2016 , 19 , 41–54. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chen, B.-B.; Han, W. Ecological Assets and Academic Procrastination among Adolescents: The Mediating Role of Commitment to Learning. Front. Psychol. 2017 , 8 , 1971. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Yang, Q.; Shi, M.; Shu, S. Academic Procrastination to Parenting Style and Achievements Motivation in Middle School Students. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 2017 , 25 , 558–560. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Korkmaz, O.; Ilhan, T.; Bardakci, S. An Investigation of Self-Efficacy, Locus of Control, and Academic Procrastination as Predictors of Academic Achievement in Students Diagnosed as Gifted and Non-Gifted. Online Submiss. 2018 , 4 , 173–192. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Macias, A.B.; Nevarez, S.B. Procrastination, and stress. Analysis of its relationship in high school students. CPU-E Rev. Investig. Educ. 2019 , 28 , 132–151. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ Green Version ]
  • Ahmadi, S.; Toulabi, S.; Ilanloo, H. The Relationship Between Tendency to Substance Abuse and Resilience and Academic Procrastination in Secondary School Students. J. Arak Univ. Med. Sci. 2020 , 23 , 108–117. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Sula Atas, N.; Kumcagiz, H. Academic Procrastination Attitudes of Adolescents, Academic Self-Efficacy Beliefs and Perfectionism Levels. Hacet. Univ. EGITIM Fak. Derg.-Hacet. Univ. J. Educ. 2020 , 35 , 375–386. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Eissa Saad, M.A.; Khalifa, A.G. Modeling Self-Regulated Learning: The Mediating Role in the Relationship between Academic Procrastination and Problematic Smartphone Use among Third Year-Middle School Learning Disabled Students. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 2020 , 18 , 507–522. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Gündüz, G.F. The Relationship between Academic Procrastination Behaviors of Secondary School Students, Learning Styles and Parenting Behaviors. Int. J. Contemp. Educ. Res. 2020 , 7 , 253–266. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Lenggono, B.; Tentama, F. Construct Measurement of Academic Procrastination of Eleventh Grade High School Students in Sukoharjo. Int. J. Sci. Technol. Res. 2020 , 9 , 454–459. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Trujillo-Chumán, K.; Noé-Grijalva, M. La Escala de Procrastinación Académica (EPA): Validez y Confiabilidad en una Muestra de Estudiantes Peruanos [The Academic Procrastination Scale: Validity and Reliability in a Sample of Peruvian Students]. Rev. Psicol. Educ. 2020 , 15 , 98–107. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Fulano, C.; Magalhães, P.; Núñez, J.C.; Marcuzzo, S.; Rosário, P. As the Twig Is Bent, so Is the Tree Inclined: Lack of Prior Knowledge as a Driver of Academic Procrastination. Int. J. Sch. Educ. Psychol. 2021 , 9 , S21–S33. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Niu, Y.; Jiang, N.; Jiang, X. Factors Related to the Resilience of Tibetan Adolescent Survivors Ten Years after the Yushu Earthquake. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2021 , 65 , 102554. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Opdenakker, M.C. Need-Supportive and Need-Thwarting Teacher Behavior: Their Importance to Boys’ and Girls’ Academic Engagement and Procrastination Behavior. Front. Psychol. 2021 , 12 , 628064. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Rezaei, S.; Zebardast, A. The Mediating Role of Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies on Mindfulness, Anxiety, and Academic Procrastination in High Schoolers. J. Pract. Clin. Psychol. 2021 , 9 , 133–142. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Selçuk, Ş.; Koçak, A.; Mouratidis, A.; Michou, A.; Sayıl, M. Procrastination, Perceived Maternal Psychological Control, and Structure in Math Class: The Intervening Role of Academic Self-concept. Psychol. Sch. 2021 , 58 , 1782–1798. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Wang, Y. Academic Procrastination and Test Anxiety: A Cross-Lagged Panel Analysis. Aust. J. Guid. Couns. 2021 , 31 , 122–129. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Yang, Z.Y. Does Procrastination Always Predict Lower Life Satisfaction? A Study on the Moderation Effect of Self-Regulation in China and the United Kingdom. Front. Psychol. 2021 , 12 , 690838. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Yu, Y.; Hua, L.; Feng, X.; Wang, Y.; Yu, Z.; Zi, T.; Zhao, Y.; Li, J. True Grit in Learning Math: The Math Anxiety-Achievement Link Is Mediated by Math-Specific Grit. Front. Psychol. 2021 , 12 , 645793. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ] [ PubMed ]
  • Asmali, M.; Sayin, S.D. Academic Procrastination in Language Learning: Adolescent Learners’ Perspectives. Acuity J. Engl. Lang. Pedagogy Lit. Cult. 2022 , 7 , 220–235. [ Google Scholar ]
  • Chaji, S.; Ebrahimpour, M.; Pakdaman, M.; Taheri, H. Study of Psychometric Properties of Problem-Solving Skills Questionnaire Heppner and Petersen in Primary School Students of Birjand. J. Psychol. 2022 , 21 , 1851–1866. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]
  • Galindo-Contreras, J.; Olivas-Ugarte, L.O. Tuckman Procrastination Scale (ATPS): Psychometric Evidence and Normative Data in Secondary School Students from Lima, Peru. Propos. Represent. 2022 , 10 , e1381. [ Google Scholar ] [ CrossRef ]

Click here to enlarge figure

CriteriaInclusionExclusion
Documents (d)Journal articles, books, book chapters, and doctoral thesesMagazine articles, editorials, conferences, etc.
Concept (Co)Academic procrastinationThe rest
Context (Co)Academic contextOther contexts (e.g., general, work or health procrastination)
Studies (s)Empirical studiesTheoretical reviews and case studies
Participants (p)Students under 18 yearsUniversity students or community samples
Evaluation (e)Behavioral or reported procrastination measures (i.e., self-reports or hetero reports)Single-item instruments or unstructured instruments
InstrumentCount
Procrastination Assessment Scale-Student—PASS (Solomon and Rothblum, 1984 [ ])23
Academic Procrastination Scale—AP-S (Çakıcı, 2003 [ ])10
Tuckman Procrastination Scale—TPS (Tuckman, 1991 [ ])8
Aitken Procrastination Inventory—API (Aiken, 1982 [ ])5
General Procrastination Scale—GPS (Lay, 1986 [ ])5
Escala de Procrastinación Académica—EPA (Busko, 1998 [ ])4
Academic Procrastination Questionnaire (Huang, 2009 [ ])2
Scale developed by authors (Dietz et al., 2007 [ ])3
Academic Procrastination Scale—APS-S (McCloskey, 2011 [ ])2
Academic Procrastination Inventory for Middle School Students—API-MSS (Zuo, 2020 [ ])1
Academic Procrastination Questionnaire (Ran, H., 2010 [ ])1
Academic Procrastination Scale—MSLQ (Lay and Silverman, 1996 [ ])1
Academic Procrastination Scale—APS (Lay, 1986 [ ])1
Academic Procrastination Student Form—APS (Milgram and Amir, 1998 [ ])1
Academic Procrastination Survey (Savari, K., 2011 [ ])1
Cuestionario de Procrastinación en el Estudio (CPE; Rosário et al., [ ])3
Irrational Procrastination Scale—IPS (Steel, 2010 [ ])1
Melbourne Decision Making Questionnaire (five items procrastination) (Mann et al., 1997 [ ])1
Scale developed by authors (Depreeuw and Lens, 1998 [ ])2
Scale developed by authors (Santyasa et al., 2020 [ ])1
Scale developed by authors (Shih, 2016 [ ])
Scale developed by authors (Ocak and Karatas, 2019 [ ])1
Academic Procrastination Scale—DPS (Ferrari et al., 1995 [ ])1
Total79
The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

González-Brignardello, M.P.; Sánchez-Elvira Paniagua, A.; López-González, M.Á. Academic Procrastination in Children and Adolescents: A Scoping Review. Children 2023 , 10 , 1016. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10061016

González-Brignardello MP, Sánchez-Elvira Paniagua A, López-González MÁ. Academic Procrastination in Children and Adolescents: A Scoping Review. Children . 2023; 10(6):1016. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10061016

González-Brignardello, Marcela Paz, Angeles Sánchez-Elvira Paniagua, and M. Ángeles López-González. 2023. "Academic Procrastination in Children and Adolescents: A Scoping Review" Children 10, no. 6: 1016. https://doi.org/10.3390/children10061016

Article Metrics

Article access statistics, further information, mdpi initiatives, follow mdpi.

MDPI

Subscribe to receive issue release notifications and newsletters from MDPI journals

REVIEW article

How study environments foster academic procrastination: overview and recommendations.

\r\nFrode Svartdal*

  • 1 Department of Psychology, UiT The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø, Norway
  • 2 Evaluation of Studies and Teaching and Higher Education Research, University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany
  • 3 Department of Psychology, Paderborn University, Paderborn, Germany

Procrastination is common among students, with prevalence estimates double or even triple those of the working population. This inflated prevalence indicates that the academic environment may appear as “procrastination friendly” to students. In the present paper, we identify social, cultural, organizational, and contextual factors that may foster or facilitate procrastination (such as large degree of freedom in the study situation, long deadlines, and temptations and distractions), document their research basis, and provide recommendations for changes in these factors to reduce and prevent procrastination. We argue that increased attention to such procrastination-friendly factors in academic environments is important and that relatively minor measures to reduce their detrimental effects may have substantial benefits for students, institutions, and society.

Procrastination, voluntarily delaying tasks despite expecting to be worse off ( Steel, 2007 ), is common among students. Conservative estimates indicate that at least half of all students habitually procrastinate tasks that are important to them, such as reading for exams, writing term papers, and keeping up with weekly assignments ( Solomon and Rothblum, 1984 ; Tice and Baumeister, 1997 ; Pychyl et al., 2000 ; Schouwenburg, 2004 ; Steel, 2007 ). Consequences are negative, both for academic performance and retention ( Ellis and Knaus, 1977 ; Klassen et al., 2008 ; Zarick and Stonebraker, 2009 ; Grau and Minguillon, 2013 ; Kim and Seo, 2015 ) as well as for health and well-being ( Flett et al., 1995 ; Tice and Baumeister, 1997 ; Stöber and Joormann, 2001 ; Sirois, 2014 ).

Despite the possibility that academic environments may contribute significantly to this situation, the majority of research efforts to clarify mechanisms involved in procrastination has focused on individual variables related to personality, motivation, affect, and others (for reviews, see van Eerde, 2003 ; Steel, 2007 ; Klingsieck, 2013 ). The present paper takes a different view, focusing on situational, social, contextual, cultural, and organizational factors common in academic environments. Based on the procrastination literature, we present a selection of such factors and show how they increase the probability of procrastination. Negative effects may be general in that most students suffer. Often, however, “procrastination-friendly” factors may also affect students differentially, those being prone to procrastination in the first place being particularly vulnerable (e.g., Nordby et al., 2017 ; Visser et al., 2018 ). Thus, ideas on how to address these factors to make the academic environment more “procrastination- un friendly” are important.

We identify nine broad factors known to increase procrastination. The factors selected serve as important examples rather than an exhaustive list. For each factor, we link it to common features of academic environments, providing examples and other forms of documentation to demonstrate its significance in facilitating procrastination. We then formulate specific advice on how the negative influence of each factor may be alleviated or remedied by relatively simple structural, organizational, and educational measures.

Characteristics of Academic Procrastination

Academic procrastination occurs when a student delays work related to academic tasks ( Solomon and Rothblum, 1984 ; Tice and Baumeister, 1997 ; Pychyl et al., 2000 ; Schouwenburg, 2004 ; Steel, 2007 ). For such delays to be regarded as procrastination, the student voluntarily chooses to delay despite expecting to be worse off ( Steel, 2007 ). Thus, there is an important distinction between delays that are sensible and rational (e.g., “I chose to postpone my thesis submission because my supervisor advised me to revise the discussion part”) and those that are not (e.g., “I did not prepare for the seminar today, I watched a movie instead”). In effect, academic procrastination is a form of irrational delay, as the person acts against better judgment.

The delays seen in academic procrastination may result from late onset (e.g., “I did not start writing until just one week before deadline”) and impulsive diversions during work (e.g., “I was working, but got tired and had a coffee with a friend instead”) ( Svartdal et al., 2020 ). As is well documented in the research literature over the past 40 years, such delays and diversions are related to personality factors, as for example impulsiveness and a preference for short-term gratification, deficiencies in planning and self-regulation, low self-efficacy, tiredness, and low energy, and task avoidance ( van Eerde, 2000 ; Steel, 2007 ; Steel et al., 2018 ). The majority of this research has been correlational. Because procrastination is a complex phenomenon unfolding over time and in interaction with situational, social, contextual, cultural, and organizational factors, it is important also to focus on exogenous factors involved in this complex and dynamic phenomenon. The relative lack of such studies is unfortunate and clearly represents a gap in the procrastination field. We argue that this is particularly unfortunate in the academic area, as the student is confronted with situational, social, contextual, cultural, and organizational factors that are prone to instigate and maintain procrastination in tasks that constitute core student activities.

How Is Academic Procrastination Measured?

Academic procrastination is typically measured with self-report tools, as is general procrastination. In measuring academic procrastination, some scales focus on general tendencies to delay tasks unnecessarily, with few if any items covering academic tasks specifically. For example, the General Procrastination Scale (20 items; Lay, 1986 ), academic version, has 16 items common with the general version and four items addressing academic tasks specifically (e.g., Item 2, “I do not do assignments until just before they are to be handed in”). Similarly, the Tuckman procrastination scale (16 items; Tuckman, 1991 ) measures academic procrastination solely by general items (e.g., item 1 “I needlessly delay finishing jobs, even when they’re important”). Other academic procrastination scales focus on academic tasks exclusively, such as the Academic Procrastination State Inventory (APSI; Schouwenburg, 1995 ) and the Procrastination Assessment Scale (PASS; Solomon and Rothblum, 1984 ). The PASS contains 44 questions that address various forms of academic tasks (e.g., studying for an exam, writing a term paper) in terms of how often they are procrastinated, to which extent such procrastination represents a problem, and willingness to change.

Importantly, scores on academic procrastination scales have been validated against procrastination in real academic tasks. For example, Tuckman compared scores on his scale against actual performance points on voluntary homework assignments, where students had the opportunity to write and submit written material to gain extra course credits. He found a negative correlation, r =−0.54, between these measures, concluding that “students are well aware of their own tendencies and can report them with great accuracy” (p. 9). More recent findings (e.g., Tice and Baumeister, 1997 ; Steel et al., 2018 ) confirm a relatively close correspondence between students’ self-reported procrastination and relevant behavioral measures.

Detrimental Effects of Academic Procrastination

It is important to recognize that procrastination is not only an issue related to effective academic work. Although performance (grades) is negatively related to procrastination (for review, see Kim and Seo, 2015 ), other important problems associated with procrastination are stress, reduced well-being, and mental and physical health problems (e.g., Tice and Baumeister, 1997 ). For academic procrastination, the increased stress associated with procrastination seems to be important (e.g., Sirois, 2007 , 2014 ). Recognition of the procrastination problem as a health issue, as well as a performance issue, is imperative. In Norway, as well as in other European countries, surveys of student health indicate that an increasing number of students report psychological problems, often of serious nature. For example, in a large-scale survey among Norwegian students, the Students’ Health and Wellbeing Study ( Knapstad et al., 2018 ; N = 50,000), 29% of all students reported serious psychological problems. We do not know the role of procrastination in this situation, but it is likely that procrastination may be a contributing factor as well as a consequence. Hence, the role of the environmental factors in encouraging procrastinating is important to assess from a health perspective also.

Social and Contextual Factors Facilitating Procrastination

Rationale for selection of factors.

In the sections to come, we address situational, social, contextual, cultural, and organizational factors that are documented as facilitators of procrastination. In selection of factors, the authors first discussed a larger pool of factors and evaluated their relation to the academic situation. Then, based on expert judgment, we selected nine factors that met the following criteria: They (a) reflect well-documented research findings in the procrastination field; (b) represent factors present in the academic situation beyond the student’s control (e.g., long deadlines), or factors that cannot easily be remedied by the student independently of educational, social, or organizational measures (e.g., task aversion); and that (c) measures taken to change the factor is likely to reduce procrastination. The discussion of each factor is not intended as a complete review, as a review at this stage of research would be premature. Rather, for each factor, we highlight central findings connecting the factor to procrastination research, its relation to the academic environment, and remedies that may alleviate the detrimental effects associated with a given factor. Table 1 presents an overview of the factors discussed.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Factors reliably associated with procrastination, and their relation to the study environment.

Note that the factors are quite heterogeneous. Some factors (e.g., large degree of freedom in the study situation, long deadlines) identify organizational and structural properties of the academic environment, whereas others emphasize subjective evaluations (e.g., task aversiveness). Also note that the factors discussed may demonstrate “main effects” as most students may be affected, as well as interactive effects where individual characteristics act as moderators. For example, temptations and distractions in the academic environment may be detrimental for most students, but particularly so for individuals high in impulsivity and distractibility (e.g., Steel et al., 2018 ). Furthermore, the order of factors discussed does not indicate differences in importance. In fact, the effect sizes associated with each factor may be difficult to quantify in academic contexts. Finally, a caution on the use of the term “factor.” We use this term to denote facets or variables in the academic settings that identify features known to relate strongly to procrastination. As these are exogenous factors in the procrastination equation, they represent potential conditions that can be altered in order to affect the probability of procrastination. In the present context, we do not make strong assumptions about causality; rather, we argue that such potential causal relations should receive increased attention in future research.

Large Degree of Freedom in the Study Situation

Relevant research.

In his comprehensive review of research on procrastination, Steel (2007) coined procrastination a quintessential self-regulatory failure. Procrastinators are present-oriented and impulsive and tend to score low on tests measuring conscientiousness and planning, and high on susceptibility to temptation ( Lay and Schouwenburg, 1993 ; van Eerde, 2003 ; Steel, 2010 ). Procrastinators make plans, only to reverse them when encountering distractions and temptations during goal implementation ( Steel et al., 2018 ). Hence, procrastinators are particularly vulnerable when working under unstructured conditions and when long-term plans are delegated to the individual.

Relation to the Academic Environment

Results from qualitative studies exemplify the negative role of freedom in the study situation in several ways, as too little regulations in studies ( Grunschel et al., 2013 ), low degree of external structure ( Klingsieck et al., 2013 ), or insufficient direction of lecturers ( Patrzek et al., 2012 ). Overall, students reported feeling lost and overwhelmed by the task of planning a whole course of studies, a semester, or even an exam phase on their own. Thus, students lacking self-management skills such as planning and prioritizing tasks (e.g., Lay and Schouwenburg, 1993 ) and metacognitive learning strategies (e.g., Wolters, 2003 ; Howell and Watson, 2007 ) should feel particularly lost when facing a situation with a large degree of freedom. The autonomy associated with a large degree of freedom in the study situation makes the student particularly vulnerable if skills are low (→Low focus on study skills training) and if the student fails to develop good habits and routines. Habits help people accomplish more and procrastinate less (e.g., Steel et al., 2018 ). Of note, study topics may vary in how much freedom they offer to the student. Some study programs are strictly structured and may even involve a common study group from start to finish (e.g., medicine), whereas other study topics are less structured and may also, by the nature of their contents, appear as more “procrastination friendly” (e.g., Nordby et al., 2017 ).

While direct procrastination prevention and intervention programs train the self-management skill of students (for a summary, see van Eerde and Klingsieck, 2018 ), remedies should also be implemented on the level of study programs and the level of courses. Especially for beginning students, unnecessary options present opportunities for students to procrastinate and should be accompanied by remedial measures. For example, Ariely and Wertenbroch (2002) compared student performance under no-choice fixed working schedules determined by the teacher versus choice working schedules (the students could determine their own schedules) and found that performance was better when students had to follow the no-choice fixed working schedules. If possible, a detailed syllabus including a “timetable” of the course, all deadlines, expected learning outcomes, and resources such as literature can help downsize the large degree of freedom of a study situation (cf. Eberly et al., 2001 ). Concerning the study program, an orientation event in the first semester or even each semester might support students in seeing the program’s inherent structure. One should not only focus on the contents of the program but also on the best way to run through the program. An individual twist to the orientation could be a short workshop in which each student is encouraged to plan her or his semester, thereby downsizing the large degree of freedom by establishing a unique structure which, ideally, should take into account all other activities they wish to make time for (e.g., sports, family, job), as well. Teaching styles that support student autonomy ( Codina et al., 2018 ) may also be helpful. Finally, note that a large degree of freedom in the study situation is not alleviated by the introduction of more external control. Indeed, procrastination research demonstrates that external control is associated with increased procrastination (e.g., Janssen and Carton, 1999 ). We argue instead that unnecessary freedom should be reduced, as in the Ariely and Wertenbroch (2002) study discussed.

Long Deadlines

The idea of hyperbolic discounting helps to explain why we procrastinate the start of an activity. For example, according to the Temporal Motivation Theory (TMT; Steel and König, 2006 ; Gröpel and Steel, 2008 ), motivation increases as a function of the expectancy of an outcome and the size or value of a goal, but decreases as the time span before this outcome lengthens and impulsiveness increases. Thus, procrastination is more likely to occur if the outcome of an activity offers rewards in the distant future, and more so if impulsiveness is high (as is the case in procrastinators). Hence, immediate temptations often come to dominate over distant rewarding goals.

Results from qualitative ( Schraw et al., 2007 ) and quantitative studies ( Tice and Baumeister, 1997 ; Schouwenburg and Groenewoud, 2001 ) support the idea that the tendency to procrastinate decreases as the deadline for the task in question is approaching. Students find tentative due dates as especially frustrating ( Schraw et al., 2007 ). In the absence of deadlines, students often set deadlines for themselves. Although such deadlines may work to reduce procrastination, they may actually reduce performance ( Ariely and Wertenbroch, 2002 ). Other research, focusing on planning, has demonstrated that individuals tend to underestimate the necessary time it takes to complete tasks (the planning fallacy; Kahneman and Tversky, 1979 ; Kahneman and Lovallo, 1993 ) and to prefer longer deadlines when allowed to choose ( Solomon and Rothblum, 1984 ). Recently, Zhu et al. (2019) demonstrated that long deadlines induce an inference of the focal task as more difficult, thereby making the student to allocate more time and resources to the task. However, the downside is that such elevated resource estimates may induce longer intention-action gaps (time before starting the task) and higher likelihood of quitting.

While students with a broad range of self-management skills are able to deal with long and tentative deadline by breaking distant goals into nearer sub-goals themselves, students who lack these skills would benefit from structural arrangements defining sub-goals with timely deadlines. For instance, having students hand in an outline for a paper after the first third of the semester, the first draft after the second third, and the final draft at the end of the semester help to break a distant goal down to nearer sub-goals. Ideally, this scaffolding of self-regulating learning and writing might function as a model for future tasks with long deadlines. In general, making goals proximate (e.g., in the form of sub-goals) may help the student increase performance and reduce procrastination (e.g., Steel et al., 2018 ). Also, as reviewed by Gollwitzer and Sheeran (2006) , adapting specific implementation intentions (“if-then”-plans rather than overall goal intentions) may have a strong effect on goal attainment. When students experience difficulties in goal striving, focusing on the main obstacle hindering progress is recommended (mental contrasting; e.g., Duckworth et al., 2011 ).

Task Aversiveness

Procrastination can be understood as a form of short-term mood-regulation ( Sirois and Pychyl, 2013 ). Bad mood and negative feelings associated with a task is often repaired by avoiding the task and engaging in a pleasant task instead. The role of task aversiveness in triggering procrastination has received strong support (for a summary, see Steel, 2007 ). Closer examination of the task aversiveness literature demonstrates that aversive tasks are characterized by lower autonomy, lower task significance, boredom, resentment, frustration, and difficulty ( Milgram et al., 1988 ; Milgram et al., 1995 ; Blunt and Pychyl, 2000 ; Steel, 2007 ). Moreover, Lay (1992) found that procrastinators tend to perceive common tasks in everyday life as more aversive compared to non-procrastinators, suggesting that procrastinators face the world with a negative bias toward task execution in general. As aversive conditions tend to motivate negatively by avoidance or escape, passivity is a likely effect ( Veale, 2008 ). In sum, working under negative motivation is common in procrastinators, and a negative motivational regime is associated with passivity.

As study-related tasks typically are imposed by others (teachers, exams), they represent an important part of the academic environment for students. Such conditions are known to induce aversiveness and thereby procrastination. For example, when applying the Procrastination Assessment Scale-Students ( Solomon and Rothblum, 1984 ), one prominent dimension turns out to be aversiveness of task . Time sampling as well as daily logs also show that the more students dislike a task, the more they procrastinate ( Steel, 2007 ). Results of qualitative interview studies support these findings ( Grunschel et al., 2013 ; Klingsieck et al., 2013 ; Visser et al., 2018 ).

Why students perceive academic tasks as aversive may be traced to the fact that students entering the university often lack adequate study skills to successfully managing mastery tasks 1 . Considering academic writing, for example, The Stanford Study of Writing indicates that, for most writers, the transition from high school to college writing is enormously challenging ( Rogers, 2008 ). Moreover, university students report a variety of problems associated with academic writing (e.g., being aware of not being able to meet expected standards; Achieve Inc., 2005 ). In the last decades, universities have addressed the need for training academic writing by implementing writing centers. However, as discussed in another section (→Low focus on study skills training), instruction covering study skills is rarely provided. Thus, students often perceive academic tasks as aversive due to their lack of perceived competence. This effect may be amplified by low academic self-efficacy commonly seen in new students. Academic self-efficacy is negatively correlated to procrastination ( r = −0.44; van Eerde, 2003 ), indicating that procrastinators perceive academic tasks as even more difficult (and therefore more aversive) compared to others. Indeed, a recent study 2 found that students perceive academic tasks (e.g., present at a seminar) as more aversive compared to non-academic tasks (e.g., clean one’s apartment), but for both categories, aversiveness scores correlated positively with dispositional procrastination scores.

The Self-Determination Theory ( Deci and Ryan, 2002 ) suggests that tasks and conditions which meet a learner’s need for autonomy, competence, and relatedness support the internalization of extrinsic regulations and values, which in turn makes the task less aversive. Learners are more likely to internalize a learning goal if they embrace the meaningfulness or rationale of a task or activity if the underlying task or activity promotes their feeling of competence and if they are able to connect with other learners and experience a feeling of relatedness. Thus, formulating meaningful learning goals that lead to learning activities that fit the students’ competence level will make the task less aversive. Carefully crafted group tasks (→Inefficient group work) can also reduce procrastination. These kinds of tasks should foster the self-determination of learners. If one then embeds the learning activities in realistic learning settings, learners might even get interested in the learning activity. Game-based learning provides an innovative possibility for learning settings ( Breuer and Bente, 2010 ). Finally, as discussed elsewhere (→Low focus on study skills training), programs for students entering the university should not shy away from offering training even in the most basic study skills.

Temptations and Distractions

Individuals are tuned toward attainment of positive outcomes and escape from or avoidance of aversive events. In procrastinators, this picture is exaggerated, with current attractive and aversive events dominating over distant ones. Procrastinators tend to be impulsive and present-biased ( van Eerde, 2003 ; Steel, 2007 ), scoring high on scales measuring susceptibility to temptation, distractibility, and impulsivity ( Steel et al., 2018 ). In fact, the correlation between distractibility and procrastination is very high, r = 0.64–0.72. Thus, procrastinators are especially vulnerable to environments with an abundance of temptations and distractors, as such environments tend to capture attention and divert planned behavior into more pleasurable activities available here and now. When working with aversive tasks (→Task aversiveness), this tendency increases, as the student will be motivated to escape the aversive situation as well as divert to something attractive ( Tice et al., 2001 ).

Academic environments offer a large number of temptations and distraction, Internet access being a prime example (e.g., Reinecke and Hofmann, 2016 ). Mobile phones and laptops may have internet access everywhere on campus, presenting a continuous temptation and distractor, even during lectures. Universities tend to rely on web-based information and registration systems, and there is an increasing emphasis on digital utilities designed to assist learning, all necessitating continuous Internet access. The downside is that this situation presents a continuous challenge to students, especially those low in self-control ( Panek, 2014 ). Internet use has often been shown to conflict with other goals and obligations ( Quan-Haase and Young, 2010 ; Reinecke and Hofmann, 2016 ), and Lepp et al. (2015) demonstrated that total usage of mobile phones among undergraduates is negatively related to academic performance. Procrastination implies that the individual spends less time on focal tasks ( Lay, 1992 ), and time spent on distracting tasks add to the problems procrastinators already experience. Internet multitasking (accessing the Internet while doing something else) is positively correlated with procrastination ( Reinecke et al., 2018a , b ), indicating that procrastinators are especially prone to suffer when Internet access remains unrestricted.

Intervention studies ( Hinsch and Sheldon, 2013 ) have demonstrated that reduction in leisure-related Internet use results in decreased procrastination and increased life satisfaction. Hence, limiting the availability of Internet use is a simple way of reducing these problems. Several companies practice restriction on use of mobile phones/laptops during meetings, and universities may consider similar measures. Universities may arrange wifi-free zones for teaching and studying, and teachers may ask students to turn off their laptops/phones during classes. For many, such advice may seem counterintuitive, as the use of “modern technology” in education is generally welcomed. However, given the detrimental effects associated with unrestricted Internet use seen in the part of the student population struggling with procrastination (i.e., half or more of all students), our advice is clear.

Limited Information for Proper Self-Monitoring

In self-regulated activities, three factors are particularly important for students ( Baumeister and Heatherton, 1996 ): The student must have some standard to aim for (e.g., obtain a good grade in a course), monitor progress toward this standard, and correct as necessary if progress deviates from what is necessary to reach the standard. Although all three factors are important, Baumeister and Heatherton (1996 , p. 56) pointed out that monitoring is crucial: “Over and over, we found that managing attention was the most common and often the most effective form of self-regulation and that attentional problems presaged a great many varieties of self-regulation failure.” As procrastination is considered a prime example of a self-regulation failure ( Steel, 2007 ), it is likely that managing attention when working toward long-term goals is particularly vulnerable in procrastinators.

Due to the large degree of freedom in the study situation, the successful student needs information to keep an updated track of status, given long-term plans. Unfortunately, the study situation typically provides limited information. In many cases, exams (often held at the end of the semester) are the main source of feedback for students. Other kinds of information on progress (e.g., time spent at the university, participation in classes, observation of other students) may be unreliable as indicators of being on track. Furthermore, as consequences of procrastination are positive in the short term but not so in the longer term, learning is biased in favor of immediate positive consequences, and corrective action from long-term negative consequences is less likely.

Measures that reflect goal-striving according to plan should be implemented. From the institutional/teacher perspective, such measures should focus on reading plans, course progress, and submissions, and should not be mixed up with study performance (e.g., grades). For example, as procrastination is a reliable predictor of study effort, high procrastinators spending less time in self-directed work ( Lay, 1992 ; Svartdal et al., 2020 ), actual time spent on self-directed studying may be relevant information for many. Self-testing, recommended as an effective learning strategy (→Low focus on study skills training), also assists self-monitoring. Activity diaries, inspired by behavioral activation for depression interventions (e.g., Jacobson et al., 2001 ), may increase students’ awareness of how they spend their time as students. In recent years, several mobile apps have been developed to help students keep track of how they spend their time in the study situation (e.g., Dute et al., 2016 ), but little is known about the effect such apps may have in reducing procrastination.

Low Focus on Study Skills Training

In a qualitative study, Grunschel et al. (2013) found that students reported a lack of study skills as a notable reason for academic procrastination. One likely explanation is that low skills make tasks more effort demanding, and individuals are more likely to procrastinate on effort-demanding tasks ( Milgram et al., 1988 ). Low academic skills also make academic tasks more frustrating, boring, and difficult, which are also factors reliably associated with task aversiveness ( Blunt and Pychyl, 2000 ). As discussed in another section, task aversiveness is a reliable predictor for procrastination (→Task aversiveness).

A large part of academic work is spent on self-directed learning, and the skills needed to properly maneuver in such an environment is essential for student success ( Kreber et al., 2005 ). Unfortunately, most students have not received instruction on effective and timely study skills (e.g., Dunlosky et al., 2013 ; Dunlosky and Rawson, 2015 ), and universities are slow in implementing effective skills instruction ( Goffe and Kauper, 2014 ; Wieman and Gilbert, 2015 ). Teachers’ knowledge of effective study strategies is also lacking ( Morehead et al., 2016 ; Blasiman et al., 2017 ).

Study skill training programs produce beneficial effects in terms of academic performance and retention ( Hattie et al., 1996 ; Gettinger and Seibert, 2002 ; Robbins et al., 2004 ; Wibrowski et al., 2017 ). Moreover, studies point out that learning how to study effectively cannot be separated from course contents and the process of learning ( Weinstein et al., 2000 ; Durkin and Main, 2002 ; Wingate, 2007 ). That is, study skills training should be tailored for study programs or courses. They should suit the instructional context and teaching practices, expected achievement outcomes, and promote a high degree of learner activity. However, the impact of such skill learning interventions diminishes over time ( Wibrowski et al., 2017 ), suggesting that repetition may be crucial. Thus, dedicating a portion of instruction time or having a study skill seminar at the beginning of each semester or course may be a good strategy. Different interventions may be considered depending on the course tasks ( Schraw et al., 2007 ), students’ abilities and performance level ( Hattie et al., 1996 ). Furthermore, as knowledge of study skills are not automatically translated into good study habits, academic self-efficacy (see next section) is important for circumventing procrastination ( Klassen et al., 2008 ).

Lack of Self-Efficacy-Building Opportunities

Self-efficacy, our belief in our ability to manage a task, influences how willing we are to take on domain-specific challenges. The higher self-efficacy, the more likely we will take on a task ( Bandura and Schunk, 1981 ). Even when ability to perform a task is high, but self-efficacy for that ability is low, the likelihood of prioritizing the task goes down, and procrastination is likely ( Haycock et al., 1998 ; Klassen et al., 2008 ). Importantly, the relation between self-efficacy and procrastination is relatively strong and negative, r = −0.44 ( van Eerde, 2003 ).

Self-efficacy is one of the strongest predictors of academic performance ( Klomegah, 2007 ), yet is often neglected in course instruction. We have long known that students develop their self-efficacy for any academic task by gradually increasing proficiency with it ( Bandura, 1997 ). Furthermore, as self-efficacy tends to be context-specific and will not automatically transfer over different tasks or activities ( Zimmerman and Cleary, 2006 ), a relatively broad set of on efficacy-building experiences, course by course, is necessary (→Lack of study skill training), though not necessarily enough on its own ( Kurtovic et al., 2019 ). Other research has recently indicated that self-efficacy may be indirectly rather than directly related to academic procrastination ( Li et al., 2020 ), and that self-efficacy for self-regulation, for example, may be a strong predictor ( Zhang et al., 2018 ).

To improve self-efficacy, instructors can create more opportunities for mastery experiences by breaking down course assignments into manageable bits that are not too easy but still are possible for students to succeed at ( Bandura, 1997 ), and by helping students self-reflect on their performance such that they feel more self-efficacious in the forethought phase of subsequent work ( Zimmerman, 2000 ). As self-efficacy increases, and the likelihood of engaging in a task goes up ( Ames, 1992 ), anxiety goes down ( Haycock et al., 1998 ), establishing a virtuous circle of self-efficacy instead of a vicious circle of procrastination ( Wäschle et al., 2014 ). This can be done through in-class activities or short assignments where the goal is to scaffold student learning with positive feedback and concrete information for how to improve on increasingly challenging versions of the task ( Tuckman and Schouwenburg, 2004 ).

Inefficient Group Work

Students often work in groups (e.g., discussion groups, seminars), but often lack the basic skills for making group work effective. Group work also increases the probability of social loafing, the tendency for individuals to demonstrate less effort when working collectively than when working individually ( Karau and Williams, 1993 ). Students may therefore often prefer to work alone as an alternative. However, working alone is associated with increased procrastination ( Klingsieck et al., 2013 ). Qualitative evidence suggests that group work with interdependence between group members may reduce academic procrastination ( Klingsieck et al., 2013 ). In support, results from educational psychology have shown positive effects of interdependent group work on individual effort in settings of cooperative learning. These studies also demonstrate beneficial effects of interdependence on social support, self-esteem, and health outcomes of group members ( Johnson and Johnson, 2002 , 2009 ). Taken together, these findings indicate the potential benefit of group work with interdependence, which may be harnessed in educational settings to reduce academic procrastination.

Although the beneficial effects of student group work in higher education seem evident ( Springer et al., 1999 ; Johnson and Johnson, 2002 ), group work is neglected in curricula of many study programs, leading students to work individually on tasks and assignments and thus possibly promoting procrastination. Students in such programs may not always feel inclined to form study groups on their own and create more favorable group work conditions instead. This is especially unfortunate as methods and tools for group learning and studying abound.

Group work with interdependence may be well suited to reduce procrastination among group members. Implementing group work with interdependence should be quite straightforward, for example by having groups work on projects or by adapting individual assignments to become interdependent tasks. The latter can be achieved by designing subtasks that need to be completed sequentially by assembling groups in such a way that each member contributes unique skills, or by formulating group-level goals and rewards ( Weber and Hertel, 2007 ).

Influence of Peers

Prior research has indicated quite complex findings regarding the role of peers in facilitating or inhibiting procrastination (e.g., Nordby et al., 2017 ). Of the different ways in which peers may influence procrastination, three factors seem to be particularly important: social norms, observational learning, and distraction. Harris and Sutton (1983) suggested that an organization’s norms can either encourage or discourage procrastination, depending on whether norms suggest a prompt or delayed processing of tasks. Observational learning can support acquisition, inhibition, and triggering of many types of human behavior ( Bandura, 1985 ), including procrastination. Thus, learning from others may also influence procrastination as well as strategies against it.

With regard to social norms, Ackerman and Gross (2005) found less procrastination among students when perceived norms suggested to start promptly. Social learning of procrastination or strategies against it have not been demonstrated empirically. However, on a more general level, observational learning has been shown to influence students’ self-regulatory skills (e.g., Zimmerman and Schunk, 2004 ). Indirect support for this notion also comes from Klingsieck et al. (2013) and Nordby et al. (2017) , who report that peer behavior is taken into account by procrastinating students. With regard to social distraction, an early study reported peer influence to be a possible, yet not very frequent reason for procrastination ( Solomon and Rothblum, 1984 ). Both qualitative ( Klingsieck et al., 2013 ) and quantitative ( Chen et al., 2016 ) evidence support the idea that distraction by peers can be a source of academic procrastination. A lack of social integration has also been reported an antecedent of academic procrastination ( Patrzek et al., 2012 ), suggesting a balanced judgment on the role of peers and social contacts.

Communication of social norms to start tasks promptly can occur through regular class instruction, thus supporting timely beginning of students with a disposition to procrastinate. Social cognitive theory predicts that social learning is facilitated, among others, by the salience of both model behavior and vicarious reinforcements ( Bandura, 1985 ). Letting students reflect on and share their experiences with procrastination and strategies against it may support more productive observational learning.

This paper discusses nine factors characteristic of student study environments that, singly and in combination, increase the probability of procrastination. Clearly, given the high prevalence of academic procrastination, it is important to have an increased awareness of such risk factors and how they can be handled in order to prevent and reduce procrastination. Although we cannot control what students do, we can control how institutions encourage more productive behaviors for student success. We now briefly discuss how policymakers, universities, teachers, and students should approach these issues.

Do the Factors Point to Common Problem Areas?

Yes. We argue that the nine factors discussed can be loosely grouped into three themes (see Figure 1 ). First, four or five of the factors discussed (i.e., long deadlines, large degree of freedom in the study situation, temptations and distractions, poor self-monitoring information, and low focus on skills training), while being contextual and situational in nature, all relate directly to students’ ability to effectively self-regulate in the study situation. In effect, our overview indicates that the core problem of procrastination, poor self-regulation ( Tice et al., 2001 ; Steel, 2007 ; Hagger et al., 2010 ), is amplified by common aspects of the student environment. An important implication of this insight is that training in self-regulation techniques among students (which we recommend) should not only be tailored to the specific needs of the students (cf. Valenzuela et al., 2020 ) but should also be supplemented with specific contextual and organizational measures that can support productive self-regulation. Since it is well known that self-regulation in the academic setting is important for performance (e.g., Duckworth and Seligman, 2005 ), it is paradoxical that academic institutions organize academic student life in ways counter to this insight.

www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. How procrastination-friendly factors relate to important themes in education.

Note that the problems in self-regulation seen in procrastination episodes may relate to skills factors (e.g., planning, monitoring), speaking for relevant skills training to strengthen self-regulation. However, often factors that undermine effective self-regulation are of primary importance in procrastination (e.g., Tice et al., 2001 ). For example, low energy and tiredness may render the individual more vulnerable to task-irrelevant temptations and distractions and increase task aversiveness, which in turn increases the probability of procrastination ( Tice et al., 2001 ; Baumeister and Tierney, 2011 ). Insufficient sleep, common in the student population (e.g., Lund et al., 2010 ), is an important source of low energy and tiredness. Importantly, Knapstad et al. (2018) found that the most frequently reported health problem (as measured by the Somatic Symptoms Scale, SSS-8; Gierk et al., 2014 ) among a large sample of Norwegian students was a “Feeling of tiredness and low energy,” 45% of the students indicating that they were “fairly much or “very much” affected. This suggests that factors that undermine self-regulation among students should receive increased attention.

Second, the academic context can be designed to redress the skills and motivational issues that are often associated with procrastination. Low focus on study skills training and relative lack of efficacy-building opportunities represent a problematic combination that may themselves contribute to students perceiving academic tasks as aversive, thereby increasing the probability of procrastination. All these combined represent a disadvantageous motivational regime for academic work. The present overview identified specific organizational measures that institutions can take to change this situation. As discussed, increased focus on study skills training in concert with regular teaching may be a solution, as repeated mastery experiences will build self-efficacy as well as reduce task aversion.

Third, we should address the social factors that distract students from their academic work. By acknowledging that procrastination is a trap for students working alone, more opportunities can be made to encourage more collaborative work with others. It is important to carefully design group work in that it resembles interdependent group work. Furthermore, group work with student peers can be deliberately designed to increase student accountability, facilitating more need for self-regulation and offering students the opportunity to observe others with more productive self-regulation skills.

Given the Large Number of Factors Discussed, Are Some Particularly Important?

We have not attempted to identify effect sizes to each of the variables discussed, and for many such estimates do not exist. Comparing the factors is, therefore, extremely difficult. Further, as several of the factors discussed have been linked to procrastination in correlational research, causality must be inferred with caution. Nevertheless, all the factors discussed have potentially large causal power to instigate and sustain procrastination. Overall, the factors examined focus on larger problem areas (i.e., self-regulation, skills and motivation, social factors), but each factor identifies concrete measures to be considered to implement changes.

In approaching such factors, all should ask: What can be changed on my part? Several of the factors (e.g., large degree of freedom in the study situation, long deadlines, temptations and distractions) address organizational and educational issues that should be addressed by organizations and teachers. Others (e.g., task aversiveness) imply more complex instructor-student interactions. For example, negative emotions in task aversiveness should be approached by teachers and students in cooperation by reducing task-associated risks and imbuing the tasks with personal relevance ( van Grinsven and Tillema, 2006 ; Rowe et al., 2015 ), by enabling and encouraging student ownership of learning tasks ( Rowe et al., 2015 ), and by facilitating frequent successful learning experiences that increase self-efficacy.

Does It Make Sense to Implement Changes in One or Few Factors, Leaving Out Others?

Given an abundance of factors discussed, each capable of instigating procrastination, the high occurrence of procrastination in the student population is not at all surprising. Would it help, then, to change one or perhaps a few factors? One possible answer is that focusing on one factor is better than doing nothing. However, the downside of such an approach is that this single factor may not generate noticeable changes alone. Our recommendation would rather be to evaluate several or all factors and then implement changes as suitable within a single course, across courses, or in study programs. Note here that several of the factors discussed are relatively closely interwoven. For example, a large degree of freedom in the study situation often also implies long deadlines, suggesting that two factors may be addressed at once.

In such evaluations, it should be noted that each of the factors discussed is presented at a rather abstract level, so that relevance and concrete implementations in various settings must be carefully considered. For example, study topics vary by their very nature in how much freedom they represent for the student. Some study programs are already strictly structured and typically involve a common study group from start to finish, indicating that such programs do not need an increased focus on structure. Other programs are less structured and may also, by the nature of their study contents, be more “procrastination friendly” (e.g., Nordby et al., 2017 ). In other cases, such as study skills training and efficacy-building opportunities, “the more, the better” seems appropriate when closely linked to actual course learning tasks.

In evaluating the need for implementation of changes, the relevant factor should be assessed not only at the institutional level but—probably more importantly—at the program and course level. This applies not only to a need-based evaluation (“What do students need in order to reduce their procrastination?”), but also to a competence evaluation (“Can we provide the necessary work required for this implementation?”). Note also that some measures may be quite easy to plan on paper, but difficult to implement in a more complex system of rules and bureaucracy. For example, although long deadlines should be warned against (they induce procrastination), finding alternative solutions that can handle shorter deadline in a proper way may require changes (e.g., legal or practical) that are not easily possible to implement.

Where to Start?

In developing prevention or interventions programs concerning procrastination, one has to keep the interplay between personal factors (i.e., student characteristics) and contextual factors (i.e., institutions, courses, and teachers) in mind. As can be seen from Table 2 , the recommendations on the institutional, course, and teacher side will only fully unfold their effectiveness if students are simultaneously prepared to work on their self-regulatory skills. Thus, the recommendations we present in this paper should be accompanied by a culture of goal-focused self-regulation training programs. And, as discussed, self-regulation training programs, whether preventive or interventional, should not be administered without paying attention to contextual procrastination-friendly factors.

www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Recommended measures to reduce procrastination.

Given the high prevalence estimates of procrastination among students, a closer look at procrastination-friendly factors in the academic environment is clearly warranted. The present paper identifies nine such factors and provides suggestions on how they may be changed in order to understand, prevent, and reduce academic procrastination. Clearly, more research is needed in this area, both with regard to the factors themselves (how many are they?) as well as to their interplay and relative importance. Given the potential beneficial effects for students, institutions, and society, we conclude that researchers should pay increased attention to social, cultural, organizational, and contextual factors in their endeavors to understand academic procrastination.

Author Contributions

FS initiated the project, wrote the introduction and discussion parts. All authors contributed at least one section each to the review and edited the complete draft.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Piers Steel and Efim Nemtcan for valuable comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript. Publication charges were covered by the publication fund of UiT The Arctic University of Norway.

  • ^ We use «study skills» in a broad sense, referring to skills needed on the part of the student to successfully master various aspects of study tasks (cf. Tressel et al., 2019 ).
  • ^ Svartdal et al. (2020) . Unpublished data.

Achieve Inc. (2005). Rising to the Challenge: Are High School Graduates Prepared for College and Work?. Washington, DC: Achieve Inc.

Google Scholar

Ackerman, D. S., and Gross, B. L. (2005). My instructor made me do it: task characteristics of procrastination. J. Market. Educ. 27, 5–13. doi: 10.1177/0273475304273842

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ames, C. (1992). “Achievement goals and the classroom motivational climate,” in Student Perceptions in the Classroom , eds D. H. Schunk and J. L. Meece(Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.), 327–348.

Ariely, D., and Wertenbroch, K. (2002). Procrastination, deadlines, and performance: self-control by precommitment. Psychol. Sci. 13, 219–224. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1985). Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The Exercise of Control. New York, NY: Macmillan.

Bandura, A., and Schunk, D. H. (1981). Cultivating competence, self-efficacy, and intrinsic interest through proximal self-motivation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 41, 586–598. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.41.3.586

Baumeister, R. F., and Heatherton, T. F. (1996). Self-regulation failure: an overview. Psychol. Inq. 7, 1–15. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0701_1

Baumeister, R. F., and Tierney, J. (2011). Willpower: Rediscovering The Greatest Human Strength. New York, NY: Penguin.

Blasiman, R. N., Dunlosky, J., and Rawson, K. A. (2017). The what, how much, and when of study strategies: comparing intended versus actual study behaviour. Memory 25, 784–792. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2016.1221974

Blunt, A. K., and Pychyl, T. A. (2000). Task aversiveness and procrastination: a multi-dimensional approach to task aversiveness across stages of personal projects. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 28, 153–167. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00091-4

Breuer, J., and Bente, G. (2010). Why so serious? On the relation of serious games and learning. J. Comp. Game Cult. 4, 7–24.

Chen, B., Shi, Z., and Wang, Y. (2016). Do peers matter? Resistance to peer influence as a mediator between self-esteem and procrastination among undergraduates. Front. Psychol. 7:1529. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01529

Codina, N., Valenzuela, R., Pestana, J. V., and Gonzalez-Conde, J. (2018). Relations between student procrastination and teaching styles: autonomy-supportive and controlling. Front. Psychol. 9:809. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00809

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2002). “Overview of self-determination theory: an organismic dialectical perspective,” in Handbook of Self-Determination Research , eds E. L. Deci and R. M. Ryan (Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press), 3–33.

Duckworth, A. L., Grant, H., Loew, B., Oettingen, G., and Gollwitzer, P. M. (2011). Self-regulation strategies improve self-discipline in adolescents: benefits of mental contrasting and implementation intentions. Educ. Psychol. 31, 17–26. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2010.506003

Duckworth, A. L., and Seligman, M. E. (2005). Self-discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic performance of adolescents. Psychol. Sci. 16, 939–944. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01641.x

Dunlosky, J., and Rawson, K. A. (2015). Practice tests, spaced practice, and successive relearning: tips for classroom use and for guiding students’ learning. Scholarsh. Teach. Learn. Psychol. 1:72. doi: 10.1037/stl0000024

Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J., and Willingham, D. T. (2013). Improving students’ learning with effective learning techniques: promising directions from cognitive and educational psychology. Psychol. Sci. Publ. Interest 14, 4–58. doi: 10.1177/1529100612453266

Durkin, K., and Main, A. (2002). Discipline-based study skills support for first-year undergraduate students. Active Learn. High. Educ. 3, 24–39. doi: 10.1177/1469787402003001003

Dute, D. J., Bemelmans, W. J. E., and Breda, J. (2016). Using mobile apps to promote a healthy lifestyle among adolescents and students: a review of the theoretical basis and lessons learned. JMIR mHealth uHealth 4:e39. doi: 10.2196/mhealth.3559

Eberly, M. B., Newton, S. E., and Wiggins, R. A. (2001). The syllabus as a tool for student-centered learning. J. Gen. Educ. 50, 56–74. doi: 10.1353/jge.2001.0003

Ellis, A., and Knaus, W. (1977). Overcoming Procrastination. New York, NY: Signet.

Flett, G. L., Blankstein, K. R., and Martin, T. R. (1995). “Procrastination, negative self-evaluation, and stress in depression and anxiety,” in Procrastination and Task Avoidance: Theory, Research, and Treatment , eds J. R. Ferrari, J. L. Johnson, and W. G. McCown (New York, NY: Plenum Press), 41–46.

Gettinger, M., and Seibert, J. K. (2002). Contributions of study skills to academic competence. Schl. Psychol. Rev. 31, 350–365. doi: 10.1080/02796015.2002.12086160

Gierk, B., Kohlmann, S., Kroenke, K., Spangenberg, L., Zenger, M., Brähler, E., et al. (2014). The somatic symptom scale-8 (SSS-8): a brief measure of somatic symptom burden. JAMA Inter. Medic. 174, 399–407. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.12179

Goffe, W. L., and Kauper, D. (2014). A survey of principles instructors: why lecture prevails. J. Econ. Educ. 45, 360–375. doi: 10.1080/00220485.2014.946547

Gollwitzer, P. M., and Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: a meta-analysis of effects and processes. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 38, 69–119. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(06)38002-1

Grau, J., and Minguillon, J. (2013). When procrastination leads to dropping out: analysing students at risk. ELearn Center Res. Pap. Ser. 6, 63–74.

Gröpel, P., and Steel, P. (2008). A mega-trial investigation of goal setting, interest enhancement, and energy on procrastination. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 45, 406–411. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.05.015

Grunschel, C., Patrzek, J., and Fries, S. (2013). Exploring reasons and consequences of academic procrastination: an interview study. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 28, 841–861. doi: 10.1007/s10212-012-0143-4

Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., and Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2010). Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 136, 495–525. doi: 10.1037/a0019486

Harris, N. N., and Sutton, R. I. (1983). Task procrastination in organizations: a framework for research. Hum. Relat. 36, 987–995. doi: 10.1177/001872678303601102

Hattie, J., Biggs, J., and Purdie, N. (1996). Effects of learning skills interventions on student learning: a meta-analysis. Rev. Educ. Res. 66, 99–136. doi: 10.3102/00346543066002099

Haycock, L. A., McCarthy, P., and Skay, C. L. (1998). Procrastination in college students: the role of self-efficacy and anxiety. J. Counsel. Dev. 76, 317–324. doi: 10.1002/j.1556-6676.1998.tb02548.x

Hinsch, C., and Sheldon, K. M. (2013). The impact of frequent social internet consumption: increased procrastination and lower life satisfaction. J. Consum. Behav. 12, 496–505. doi: 10.1002/cb.1453

Howell, A. J., and Watson, D. C. (2007). Procrastination: associations with achievement goal orientation and learning strategies. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 43, 167–178. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.017

Jacobson, N. S., Martell, C. R., and Dimidjian, S. (2001). Behavioral activation treatment for depression: returning to contextual roots. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 8, 255–270. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.8.3.255

Janssen, T., and Carton, J. S. (1999). The effects of locus of control and task difficulty on procrastination. J. Genet. Psychol. 160, 436–442. doi: 10.1080/00221329909595557

Johnson, D. W., and Johnson, R. T. (2002). Social interdependence theory and university instruction – theory into practice. Swiss J. Psychol. 61, 119–129. doi: 10.1024//1421-0185.61.3.119

Johnson, D. W., and Johnson, R. T. (2009). An educational psychology success story: social interdependence theory and cooperative learning. Educ. Res. 38, 365–379. doi: 10.3102/0013189X09339057

Kahneman, D., and Lovallo, D. (1993). Timid choices and bold forecasts: a cognitive perspective on risk taking. Manag. Sci. 39, 17–31. doi: 10.1287/mnsc.39.1.17

Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1979). Intuitive prediction: biases and corrective procedures. TIMS Stud. Manag. Sci. 12, 313–327.

Karau, S. J., and Williams, K. D. (1993). Social loafing: a meta-analytic review and theoretical integration. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 65, 681–706. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.65.4.681

Kim, K. R., and Seo, E. H. (2015). The relationship between procrastination and academic performance: a meta-analysis. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 82, 26–33. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.02.038

Klassen, R. M., Krawchuk, L. L., and Rajani, S. (2008). Academic procrastination of undergraduates: low self-efficacy to self-regulate predicts higher levels of procrastination. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 33, 915–931. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2007.07.001

Klingsieck, K. B. (2013). Procrastination: when good things don’t come to those who wait. Eur. Psychol. 18, 24–34. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000138

Klingsieck, K. B., Grund, A., Schmid, S., and Fries, S. (2013). Why students procrastinate: a qualitative approach. J. Coll. Stud. Dev. 54, 397–412. doi: 10.1353/csd.2013.0060

Klomegah, R. Y. (2007). Predictors of academic performance of university students: an application of the goal efficacy model. Coll. Stud. J. 41, 407–415.

Knapstad, M., Heradstveit, O., and Sivertsen, B. (2018). Studentenes Helse- og Trivselsundersøkelse 2018. [Students’ Health and Wellbeing Study 2018]. Oslo: SiO.

Kreber, C., Castleden, H., Erfani, N., and Wright, T. (2005). Self-regulated learning about university teaching: an exploratory study. Teach. High. Educ. 10, 75–97. doi: 10.1080/1356251052000305543

Kurtovic, A., Vrdoljak, G., and Idzanovic, A. (2019). Predicting procrastination: the role of academic achievement, self-efficacy and perfectionism. Int. J. Educ. Psychol. 8, 1–26. doi: 10.17583/ijep.2019.2993

Lay, C. H. (1986). At last, my research article on procrastination. J. Res. Pers. 20, 474–495. doi: 10.1016/0092-6566(86)90127-3

Lay, C. H. (1992). Trait procrastination and the perception of person-task characteristics. J. Soc. Behav. Person. 7, 483–494.

Lay, C. H., and Schouwenburg, H. C. (1993). Trait procrastination, time management, and academic behavior. J. Soc. Behav. Pers. 8, 647–662.

Lepp, A., Barkley, J. E., and Karpinski, A. C. (2015). The relationship between cell phone use and academic performance in a sample of US college students. Sage Open 5, doi: 10.1177/2158244015573169

Li, L., Gao, H., and Xu, Y. (2020). The mediating and buffering effect of academic self-efficacy on the relationship between smartphone addiction and academic procrastination. Comput. Educ. 159:104001. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2020.104001

Lund, H. G., Reider, B. D., Whiting, A. B., and Prichard, J. R. (2010). Sleep patterns and predictors of disturbed sleep in a large population of college students. J. Adolesc. Health 46, 124–132. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.06.016

Milgram, N., Marshevsky, S., and Sadeh, C. (1995). Correlates of academic procrastination: discomfort, task aversiveness, and task capability. J. Psychol. 129, 145–155. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1995.9914954

Milgram, N. A., Sroloff, B., and Rosenbaum, M. (1988). The procrastination of everyday life. J. Res. Pers. 22, 197–212. doi: 10.1016/0092-6566(88)90015-3

Morehead, K., Rhodes, M. G., and DeLozier, S. (2016). Instructor and student knowledge of study strategies. Memory 24, 257–271. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2014.1001992

Nordby, K., Klingsieck, K., and Svartdal, F. (2017). Do procrastination-friendly environments make students delay unnecessarily? Soc. Psychol. Educ. 20, 491–512. doi: 10.1007/s11218-017-9386-x

Panek, E. (2014). Left to their own devices: college students’ “guilty pleasure” media use and time management. Commun. Res. 41, 561–577. doi: 10.1177/0093650213499657

Patrzek, J., Grunschel, C., and Fries, S. (2012). Academic procrastination: the perspective of university counsellors. Int. J. Adv. Counsel. 34, 185–201. doi: 10.1007/s10447-012-9150-z

Pychyl, T. A., Morin, R. W., and Salmon, B. R. (2000). Procrastination and the planning fallacy: an examination of the study habits of university students. J. Soc. Behav. Pers. 15, 135–150.

Quan-Haase, A., and Young, A. L. (2010). Uses and gratifications of social media: a comparison of facebook and instant messaging. Bull. Sci. Technol. Soc. 30, 350–361. doi: 10.1177/0270467610380009

Reinecke, L., and Hofmann, W. (2016). Slacking off or winding down? An experience sampling study on the drivers and consequences of media use for recovery versus procrastination. Hum. Commun. Res. 42, 441–461. doi: 10.1111/hcre.12082

Reinecke, L., Meier, A., Aufenanger, S., Beutel, M. E., Dreier, M., Quiring, O., et al. (2018a). Permanently online and permanently procrastinating? The mediating role of Internet use for the effects of trait procrastination on psychological health and well-being. New Media Soc. 20, 862–880. doi: 10.1177/1461444816675437

Reinecke, L., Meier, A., Beutel, M. E., Schemer, C., Stark, B., Wölfling, K., et al. (2018b). The relationship between trait procrastination, Internet use, and psychological functioning: results from a community sample of German adolescents. Front. Psychol. 9:913. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00913

Robbins, S. B., Lauver, K., Le, H., Davis, D., Langley, R., and Carlstrom, A. (2004). Do psychosocial and study skill factors predict college outcomes? A meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 130, 261–288. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.2.261

Rogers, P. M. (2008). The Development of Writers and Writing Abilities: A Longitudinal Study Across and Beyond the College-Span. Doctoral dissertation. Oakland, CA: University of California.

Rowe, A. D., Fitness, J., and Wood, L. N. (2015). University student and lecturer perceptions of positive emotions in learning. Int. J. Qual. Stud. Educ. 28, 1–20. doi: 10.1080/09518398.2013.847506

Schouwenburg, H. C. (1995). “Academic procrastination,” in Procrastination and Task Avoidance: Theory, Research, and Treatment , eds J. R. Ferrari, J. L. Johnson, and W. G. McCown (New York, NY: Plenum Press), 71–96. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4899-0227-6_4

Schouwenburg, H. C. (2004). “Procrastination in academic settings: general introduction,” in Counseling the Procrastinator in Academic Settings , eds H. C. Schouwenburg, C. H. Lay, T. A. Pychyl, and J. R. Ferrari (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 3–17. doi: 10.1037/10808-001

Schouwenburg, H. C., and Groenewoud, J. T. (2001). Study motivation under social temptation; effects of trait procrastination. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 30, 229–240. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00034-9

Schraw, G., Wadkins, T., and Olafson, L. (2007). Doing the things we do: a grounded theory of academic procrastination. J. Educ. Psychol. 99, 12–25. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.99.1.12

Sirois, F. M. (2007). “I’ll look after my health, later”: a replication and extension of the procrastination–health model with community-dwelling adults. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 43, 15–26. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.11.003

Sirois, F. M. (2014). Procrastination and stress: exploring the role of self-compassion. Self Ident. 13, 128–145. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2013.763404

Sirois, F. M., and Pychyl, T. (2013). Procrastination and the priority of short-term mood regulation: consequences for future self. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Comp. 7, 115–127. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12011

Solomon, L. J., and Rothblum, E. D. (1984). Academic procrastination: frequency and cognitive-behavioral correlates. J. Counsel. Psychol. 31, 503–509. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.31.4.503

Springer, L., Stanne, M. E., and Donovan, S. S. (1999). Effects of small-group learning on undergraduates in science, mathematics, engineering, and technology: a meta-analysis. Rev. Educ. Res. 69, 21–51. doi: 10.3102/00346543069001021

Steel, P. (2007). The nature of procrastination: a meta-analytic and theoretical review of quintessential self-regulatory failure. Psychol. Bull. 133, 65–94. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.65

Steel, P. (2010). Arousal, avoidant, and decisional procrastinators: do they exist? Pers. Indiv. Differ. 48, 926–934. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.02.025

Steel, P., and König, C. J. (2006). Integrating theories of motivation. Acad. Manag. Rev. 3, 889–913. doi: 10.5465/amr.2006.22527462

Steel, P., Svartdal, F., Thundiyil, T., and Brothen, T. (2018). Examining procrastination across multiple goal stages: a longitudinal study of temporal motivation theory. Front. Psychol. 9:327. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00327

Stöber, J., and Joormann, J. (2001). Worry, procrastination, and perfectionism: differentiating amount of worry, pathological worry, anxiety, and depression. Cogn. Ther. Res. 25, 49–60. doi: 10.1023/A:1026474715384

Svartdal, F., Klingsieck, K. B., Steel, P., and Gamst-Klaussen, T. (2020). Measuring implemental delay in procrastination: separating onset and sustained goal striving. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 156:109762. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.109762

Tice, D. M., and Baumeister, R. F. (1997). Longitudinal study of procrastination, performance, stress, and health: the costs and benefits of dawdling. Psychol. Sci. 8, 454–458. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00460.x

Tice, D. M., Bratslavsky, E., and Baumeister, R. F. (2001). Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: if you feel bad, do it! J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 80, 53–67. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.1.53

Tressel, T., Lajoie, S. P., and Duffy, M. C. (2019). A guide for study terminology: reviewing a fragmented domain. Can. Psychol. 60, 115–127. doi: 10.1037/cap0000138

Tuckman, B. W. (1991). The development and concurrent validity of the procrastination scale. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 51, 473–480. doi: 10.1177/0013164491512022

Tuckman, B. W., and Schouwenburg, H. C. (2004). “Behavioral interventions for reducing procrastination among university students,” in Counseling the Procrastinator in Academic Setting , eds H. C. Shouwenburg, C. H. Lay, T. A. Pychl, and J. R. Ferrari (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 91–103. doi: 10.1037/10808-007

Valenzuela, R., Codina, N., Castillo, I., and Pestana, J. V. (2020). Young university students’ academic self-regulation profiles and their associated procrastination: autonomous functioning requires self-regulated operations. Front. Psychol. 11:354. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00354

van Eerde, W. (2000). Procrastination: self-regulation in initiating aversive goals. Appl. Psychol. 49, 372–389. doi: 10.1111/1464-0597.00021

van Eerde, W. (2003). A meta-analytically derived nomological network of procrastination. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 35, 1401–1418. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00358-6

van Eerde, W., and Klingsieck, K. B. (2018). Overcoming procrastination? A meta-analysis of intervention studies. Educ. Res. Rev. 25, 73–85. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2018.09.002

van Grinsven, L., and Tillema, H. (2006). Learning opportunities to support student self-regulation: comparing different instructional formats. Educ. Res. 48, 77–91. doi: 10.1080/00131880500498495

Veale, D. (2008). Behavioural activation for depression. Adv. Psychiatr. Treat. 14, 29–36. doi: 10.1192/apt.bp.107.004051

Visser, L., Korthagen, F. A., and Schoonenboom, J. (2018). Differences in learning characteristics between students with high, average, and low levels of academic procrastination: students’ views on factors influencing their learning. Front. Psychol. 9:808. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00808

Wäschle, K., Allgaier, A., Lachner, A., Fink, S., and Nückles, M. (2014). Procrastination and self-efficacy: tracing vicious and virtuous circles in self-regulated learning. Learn. Instruct. 29, 103–114. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.09.005

Weber, B., and Hertel, G. (2007). Motivation gains of inferior group members: a meta-analytical review. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 93, 973–993. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.93.6.973

Weinstein, C. E., Husman, J., and Dierking, D. R. (2000). “Self-regulation interventions with a focus on learning strategies,” in Handbook of Self-Regulation , eds M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, and M. Zeidner (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 727–747. doi: 10.1016/b978-012109890-2/50051-2

Wibrowski, C. R., Matthews, W. K., and Kitsantas, A. (2017). The role of a skills learning support program on first-generation college students’ self-regulation, motivation, and academic achievement: a longitudinal study. J. Coll. Stud. Retent. Res. Theory Pract. 19, 317–332. doi: 10.1177/1521025116629152

Wieman, C., and Gilbert, S. (2015). Taking a scientific approach to science education, Part II-Changing teaching. Microbe 10, 203–207. doi: 10.1128/microbe.10.203.1

Wingate, U. (2007). A framework for transition: supporting ‘learning to learn’ in higher education. High. Educ. Q. 61, 391–405. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2273.2007.00361.x

Wolters, C. A. (2003). Understanding procrastination from a self-regulated learning perspective. J. Educ. Psychol. 95, 179–187. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.179

Zarick, L. M., and Stonebraker, R. (2009). I’ll do it tomorrow: the logic of procrastination. Coll. Teach. 57, 211–215. doi: 10.1080/87567550903218687

Zhang, Y., Dong, S., Fang, W., Chai, X., Mei, J., and Fan, X. (2018). Self-efficacy for self-regulation and fear of failure as mediators between self-esteem and academic procrastination among undergraduates in health professions. Adv. Health Sci. Educ. 23, 817–830. doi: 10.1007/s10459-018-9832-3

Zhu, M., Bagchi, R., and Hock, S. J. (2019). The mere deadline effect: why more time might sabotage goal pursuit. J. Consum. Res. 45, 1068–1084. doi: 10.1093/jcr/ucy030

Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). “Attaining self-regulation: a social cognitive perspective,” in Handbook of Self-Regulation , eds M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, and M. Zeidne (Cambridge, MA:Academic Press), 13–39. doi: 10.1016/B978-012109890-2/50031-7

Zimmerman, B., and Schunk, D. (2004). “Self-regulating intellectual processes and outcomes: a social cognitive perspective,” in Motivation, Emotion and Cognition, Integrative Perspectives on Intellectual Functioning and Development , eds D. Dai and R. Sternberg (Abingdon: Routledge), 323–349.

Zimmerman, B. J., and Cleary, T. J. (2006). “Adolescents’ development of personal agency,” in The Role of Self-Efficacy Belief and Self-Regulatory Skill , eds F. Pajares and T. Urdan (Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing).

Keywords : academic procrastination, study environments, social factors, self-regulation, impulsivity, task aversiveness

Citation: Svartdal F, Dahl TI, Gamst-Klaussen T, Koppenborg M and Klingsieck KB (2020) How Study Environments Foster Academic Procrastination: Overview and Recommendations. Front. Psychol. 11:540910. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.540910

Received: 06 March 2020; Accepted: 12 October 2020; Published: 02 November 2020.

Reviewed by:

Copyright © 2020 Svartdal, Dahl, Gamst-Klaussen, Koppenborg and Klingsieck. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY) . The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Frode Svartdal, [email protected]

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Open access
  • Published: 03 July 2024

Predictive analysis of college students’ academic procrastination behavior based on a decision tree model

  • Pu Song 1 ,
  • Xiangwei Liu 2 ,
  • Xuan Cai 3 ,
  • Mengmeng Zhong 4 ,
  • Qingqing Wang 5 &
  • Xiangmei Zhu 6  

Humanities and Social Sciences Communications volume  11 , Article number:  869 ( 2024 ) Cite this article

739 Accesses

Metrics details

Predicting academic procrastination among college students in the context of a public crisis could provide essential academic support and decision-making strategies for higher education institutions to promote student psychological health. Notably, research focusing on predicting academic procrastination behavior among college students in the context of a global crisis is still limited. The purpose of this study is to address this gap by constructing a predictive model based on the decision tree algorithm to predict academic procrastination behavior among college students. A total of 776 college students from the Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region of China participated in this study. The study gathered data from multiple aspects relevant to academic procrastination behavior, including demographic information, academic achievements, subjective well-being, smartphone addiction, negative emotions, self-esteem, life autonomy, pro-environmental behavior, academic achievement, and sense of school belonging. Descriptive statistical analysis was conducted utilizing SPSS version 26.0, and decision tree model analysis was performed with Modeler 18.0. The findings of this study identified eight predictive factors of college students’ academic procrastination in order of importance: subjective well-being, smartphone addiction, negative emotions, self-esteem, life autonomy, pro-environmental behavior, academic performance, and sense of school belonging. The model accuracy was 85.78%, and indicating a relatively high level of prediction. The findings of this study not only provided a new perspective for understanding academic procrastination but also offered practical guidance for educators on how to mitigate this behavior.

Similar content being viewed by others

academic procrastination literature review

Methods for measuring career readiness of high school students: based on multidimensional item response theory and text mining

academic procrastination literature review

The mediating role of ego depletion in the relationship between state anxiety and academic procrastination among University students

academic procrastination literature review

A serial mediation model reveals the association between parental over-protection and academic entitlement among nursing students

Introduction.

The recent occurrence of public crisis, such as COVID-19, has caused unprecedented impacts on all levels of education (Toquero, 2020 ), particularly affecting the psychological well-being and learning of college students. The challenge of COVID-19 not only disrupts students’ academic and daily lives (Gamage et al., 2020 ; Tarkar, 2020 ) but also exacerbates academic procrastination problems. Academics further highlight that factors such as psychological well-being, life satisfaction, and learning environment contribute to the changes in academic procrastination behavior during COVID-19 (Arifiana et al., 2020 ; Peixoto et al., 2021 ; Svartdal et al., 2020 ). Therefore, investigating the predictive factors and underlying mechanisms behind college students’ academic procrastination behavior is not only crucial for better understanding their psychological changes and behavioral adjustments, but also critical for developing effective strategies to improve academic performance and well-being.

Academic procrastination behavior can be attributed to a combination of biological, psychological, and social factors, highlighting the multiple aspects and complexity of this phenomenon. It is not that uncommon for students to experience negative emotions such as anxiety and depression during the pandemic. These negative emotions not only affect the psychological well-being of students but could also lead to an increase in academic procrastination behavior (Bolbolian et al., 2021 ; Deng et al., 2022 ). Conversely, self-esteem and life autonomy can enhance intrinsic motivation and resilience when facing challenges and difficulties, thereby reducing the likelihood of procrastination behavior during crises (Tian et al., 2023 ; Yang et al., 2021 ). Moreover, subjective well-being may serve as an alleviating factor that helps students to stay positive and reduce procrastination behavior (García-Ros et al., 2023 ). Addictive behavior related to mobile phones is not new in the age of information technology. However, it is noteworthy that depression and anxiety are significantly linked with smartphone addiction, particularly during public crises, and are further associated with academic procrastination behavior (Albursan et al., 2022 ; Ratan et al., 2021 ). Excessive reliance on smartphones for information and social interaction could distract students’ attention and diminish learning efficiency, thereby worsening academic procrastination behavior (Albursan et al., 2022 ).

Pro-environmental behavior reflects a sense of social responsibility and organizational ability, which are particularly important for maintaining academic discipline and reducing tendencies toward academic procrastination (Yuriev et al., 2020 ). Studies posit that a sense of school belonging could reduce academic procrastination tendencies. Dong and Izadpanah ( 2022 ) found in their study that a strong sense of school belonging can provide emotional support and enhance social connections. These social interactions and supports promote a positive perception of the learning environment and thus mitigate academic procrastination behavior. Therefore, it can be seen that the interaction between personal and environmental factors has a significant impact on academic procrastination behavior including the time of crises. Furthermore, academic achievement is a significant predictor of academic procrastination as it reflects learning efficiency and motivation (Wu et al., 2020 ). Students who achieve high scores typically possess greater self-efficacy and goal orientation, thereby reducing tendencies to procrastinate (Goroshit & Hen, 2021 ).

A thorough investigation of the impact of these predictors on academic procrastination will enrich the understanding of the complex interplay of procrastination behavior, and provide insights for designing effective interventions. Academic procrastination is a problem to tackle even before the pandemic took place. It is important to note that this study contributes to understanding academic procrastination behavior during pandemic situations. Despite the pandemic subsiding now, stakeholders in education could still leverage these insights to better prepare for future crises beyond the pandemic era. Notably, prior studies that utilize the decision tree method to predict academic procrastination are still scarce in the literature. The decision tree model is one of the algorithms of machine learning for data mining (Sharma & Kumar, 2016 ). It involves classification or prediction based on attribute tests and outcomes (Wu et al., 2008 ).

The decision tree model has undergone several accuracy simplifications and improvements by Ross Quinlan ( 1987 , 1996 ). The merits of the decision tree model lie not only in its high predictive accuracy but also in its strength to decompose a complex decision-making process into a series of simpler decisions, thereby providing a solution that is generally easier to interpret (Quinlan, 1987 , 1996 ). For instance, previous studies have utilized decision tree models to identify innovative behavior (Safavian & Landgrebe, 1991 ; Skrbinjek & Dermol, 2019 ), and have accurately predicted factors influencing innovation success and failure (Park, 2019 ). In addition, decision tree models have been applied across various disciplines of research including marketing and psychology, to predict consumer satisfaction, attitudes, and loyalty (Han et al., 2019 ). Hence, the decision tree method is appropriate for this study to predict college students’ academic procrastination behavior and to identify variables that can serve as predictors of academic procrastination.

Literature review

Academic procrastination.

Academic procrastination was first proposed by Solomon and Rothblum in 1984, they defined it as the act of postponing planned tasks and persistently delaying or deferring task completion within the academic context (Solomon & Rothblum, 1984 ; Svartdal et al., 2020 ). However, the academic community has not yet formed a unified definition or conceptualization of academic procrastination. The majority of scholars agree that academic procrastination typically refers to the behavior of postponing and delaying mandatory academic tasks without sufficient justifications (Ferrari & Camilleri, 2021 ). Some scholars further refine this definition by suggesting that academic procrastination is the behavior of choosing to delay task completion despite being well aware of the negative consequences (Steel, 2007 ; Wang et al., 2021 ).

Apart from that, researchers also postulate that academic procrastination consists of different dimensions such as behavior, cognition, and emotion (Saplavska & Jerkunkova, 2018 ). This is mainly characterized by cognitive distortions and irrational delays in initiating or completing academic tasks (Schouwenburg, 1995 ). Cai ( 2017 ) also endorsed Schouwenburg’s viewpoint and considered academic procrastination as the discrepancy between task planning and implementation that involves irrational delays in planning or completing academic tasks as well as remediation and summary after delays. Based on the above discussions, this study defines academic procrastination as the delaying behaviour exhibited by students during academic tasks. This behavior primarily involves intentionally postponing or avoiding the completion of academic tasks, and may or may not result in failing to complete the given tasks within specified timeframes.

The predictor of academic procrastination

Extensive literature suggests that researchers are deeply interested in understanding the influencing factors of academic procrastination (Abdi Zarrin & Gracia, 2020 ; Nwosu et al., 2020 ; Wang et al., 2021 ). The selection of predictors in this study is grounded in the Biopsychosocial Model and a review of the literature, which includes subjective well-being, smartphone addiction, negative emotions, self-esteem, life autonomy, pro-environmental behavior, academic achievement, and sense of school belonging. George Engel ( 1981 ) proposed a Biopsychosocial Model to explain the impact of psychological and social factors on individual behavior. During public crises, this model can be particularly useful in understanding how students’ behaviors, psychological reactions, and social environments interact and influence their academic procrastination and well-being.

Subjective well-being typically encompasses indicators such as life satisfaction, positive affect, and negative affect (Yilmaz, 2023 ). The prolonged lockdowns and isolation during the pandemic have had a significant negative impact on students’ subjective well-being (Foa et al., 2020 ), and this could lead to negative emotions, thereby increasing academic procrastination behavior (Vlachopanou & Karagiannopoulou, 2022 ). Previous studies have shown that students with higher subjective well-being demonstrate stronger self-regulation and resilience, allowing them to manage their learning behavior and time more effectively, thereby reducing procrastination behavior (Bu, Wu, & Wang, 2021 ; Morosanova et al., 2021 ; Yadav et al., 2020 ). Recent research has also endorsed the negative correlation between university students’ subjective well-being and academic procrastination (García-Ros et al., 2023 ). The literature underscores the prediction of subjective well-being on academic procrastination by highlighting its role in improving self-regulation and resilience, which in turn significantly reduces academic procrastination. Therefore, this study proposes:

H1: Subjective well-being negatively predicts academic procrastination.

Literature suggests that smartphone addiction is one of the reasons for academic procrastination behavior (Albursan et al., 2022 ). As of the year 2021, China ranked first globally in terms of smartphone users with the number surpassing 910 million (Horani & Dong, 2023 ). Studies also indicate that during the COVID-19 lockdown, the usage time of smartphones significantly increased (Katsumata et al., 2022 ; Statista, 2021 ). A study conducted by Chen et al. ( 2017 ) reveal that the smartphone addiction rate among university student in China is 29.8%. It is expected that the percentage will continue to increase due to social isolation and increased screen time during the pandemic. Hence, this study posits smartphone addiction is one of the significant predictors of academic procrastination. Studies have also discovered that poor self-regulation is one of the contributing factors to procrastination behavior, with a significant correlation found between poor self-regulation ability and smartphone addiction (Ching & Tak, 2017 ; Zhang & Wu, 2020 ).

This is because smartphone addiction can disrupt cognitive processes and impair brain regions associated with self-regulation (Gao et al., 2020 ), leading to poor self-regulation in academic tasks and subsequent procrastination behavior (Liu et al., 2022 ). Additionally, students with smartphone addiction often experience sleep deprivation, which can contribute to a decline in cognitive function, making it more challenging to complete academic tasks on time and thus leading to academic procrastination (Cui et al., 2021 ; Hamvai et al., 2023 ). The findings from Akinci’s ( 2021 ) study also supported that smartphone addiction is an important positive predictor of academic procrastination, as students addicted to smartphones tend to neglect and procrastinate academic responsibilities. The literature highlights smartphone addiction as a key factor in academic procrastination during the pandemic. Smartphone addiction is related to sleep deprivation, poor self-regulation, impaired cognitive function, and ultimately predicting academic procrastination. Therefore, this study proposes:

H2: Smartphone addiction positively predicts academic procrastination.

It is common for students to experience negative emotions such as fear, anxiety, and stress during crises. Albujar Moreno, Castro Portillas ( 2020 ) found that individuals with higher levels of anxiety, tend to have lower self-regulation abilities and are more prone to procrastination behavior. Research indicates that negative emotions such as stress and anxiety can cause structural and functional changes in the brain by impairing memory and cognitive functions. This includes planning and goal-directed actions that could intensify procrastination tendencies (Limone et al., 2020 ; Podlesek et al., 2021 ). Being the country with the longest lockdown period, Chinese students encounter heavier academic tasks and learning pressures, making them more susceptible to negative emotions and predicting academic procrastination (Deng et al., 2022 ). Studies conducted on the detection of negative emotions among university students indicated a significant increase from 12.94% before the onset of the pandemic to 46.6% during the pandemic (Feng, 2018 ; MENG, 2020 ). The findings, revealing approximately a fourfold increase in negative emotions among students, indicate a potential connection between negative emotions and academic procrastination. Therefore, this study proposes:

H3: Negative emotions positively predict academic procrastination.

Self-esteem refers to a psychological state that evaluates one’s own worth and abilities, which can have a profound impact during public crises. Maxwell ( 1992 ) found that self-esteem is more vulnerable and significantly decreases during public crises. Academics propose that self-esteem is one of the important factors that predict academic procrastination. Shu et al. ( 2022 ) highlighted that during the COVID-19 pandemic, factors such as isolation, social restrictions, and future uncertainty affected university students’ self-esteem levels by inducing feelings of isolation, anxiety, and stress. Low self-esteem could intensify negative emotions and feelings of pressure by making students more inclined to adopting academic procrastination to escape from learning tasks and pressure (Batool, 2020 ). Similarly, a longitudinal study conducted by Yang et al. ( 2021 ) on university students reports that as self-esteem continues to decline, academic procrastination worsens progressively. The literature highlights a significant link between self-esteem and academic procrastination, emphasizing the importance of addressing self-esteem during crises to mitigate procrastination behavior. Therefore, this study proposes:

H4: Self-esteem negatively predicts academic procrastination.

Tian et al. ( 2023 ) define life autonomy is the ability of an individual to independently guide own life direction, make choices for oneself, and take responsibility for the consequences of those choices. Life autonomy encompasses several dimensions, including autonomy, self-management, and a sense of life responsibility. Greater life autonomy promotes intrinsic motivation and increases learning engagement, thus reducing academic procrastination (Guay, 2022 ; Sobia et al., 2021 ). In fact, many Chinese universities adjusted or delayed their schedules due to the pandemic, despite granting students with more flexible time. However, students with immature mindsets often struggle to engage in autonomous learning (Town et al., 2022 ). They tend to exhibit academic laziness, poor self-management skills, and procrastinate on academic tasks (Albursan et al., 2022 ; Shim et al., 2022 ). Additionally, previous studies on university students also suggest that the stronger their sense of responsibility, the less likely they are to engage in academic procrastination (Jiao & Guo, 2020 ). Evidence from the literature underscores the predictive effect of life autonomy on academic procrastination. Hence, drawing from the research on various dimensions of life autonomy and their correlation with academic procrastination, this study proposes:

H5: Life autonomy negatively predicts academic procrastination.

Procrastination can be conceptualized as an irrational tendency to delay required tasks or assignments (Klingsieck, 2013 ). Early research from Lillemo ( 2014 ) has revealed the negative correlation between pro-environmental behavior and procrastination tendencies. Studies highlight that pro-environment behavior is an important indicator for psychological regulation and adaptation from social changes and environmental crises (Colombo et al., 2023 ; Mi et al., 2021 ; Zebardast & Radaei, 2022 ). Literature implies that students who are more engaged in pro-environmental behavior also possess various qualities and traits that could negatively predict academic procrastination tendency. For example, individuals who engage in pro-environmental behavior often possess greater self-regulation, goal-setting, monitoring, and self-motivation qualities. These qualities can help individuals manage learning tasks and time more effectively, thereby negatively predicting procrastination (Colombo et al., 2023 ; Sawitri et al., 2015 ; Wang et al., 2021 ). Furthermore, implementing pro-environmental behavior requires individuals to have strong executive and organizational skills, which are opposed to academic procrastination behavior (Colombo et al., 2023 ; Gutiérrez-García et al., 2020 ). The literature review indicates that pro-environment behavior is important for psychological adaptation to environmental challenges, and suggests that individuals who exhibit pro-environmental behavior also often possess qualities and skills that mitigate academic procrastination. Therefore, this study proposes:

H6: Pro-environmental behavior negatively predicts academic procrastination.

Academic achievement is an important indicator of learning outcomes, and the academic environment has a crucial role to play. Academic achievement serves as a crucial predictor in understanding academic procrastination behavior, as it not only reflects students’ performance but also influences their psychological state and behavioral patterns (Samson, 2021 ). Recent studies suggest that the academic interruptions caused by COVID-19 have had a significant negative impact on academic achievement (Kuhfeld et al., 2020 ; Serrano Aguirre, 2020 ). Academics are interested in investigating the relationship between academic achievement and procrastination behavior, with findings supporting a negative correlation between achievement and academic procrastination (Karataş, 2015 ; Türel & Dokumacı, 2022 ). Kurtovic, Vrdoljak, and Idzanovic ( 2019 ) explain the relationship between academic achievement and procrastination by considering the level of self-efficacy. They argue that students with better academic achievement are likely to experience a greater sense of self-efficacy, negatively predicting academic procrastination. Conversely, low self-efficacy is often associated with poor academic achievement and is likely to increase procrastination behavior. The literature supports that academic achievement is a significant factor in procrastination behavior, as it reflects students’ performance and influences their psychological state and patterns of behavior. Therefore, this study proposes:

H7: Academic achievement negatively predicts academic procrastination.

During the COVID-19 pandemic, most countries in the world enforced lockdown and isolation measures, with China having the longest-lasting lockdown policies (Yu et al., 2022 ). Throughout this period, many Chinese universities conducted their courses and exams only through online platforms. The sudden transition from physical classes to online learning posed significant challenges in promoting and sustaining students’ sense of school belonging (Gopalan et al., 2022 ). Existing research has revealed that a sense of school belonging can have a significant impact on academic procrastination (Dong & Izadpanah, 2022 ; Tian et al., 2023 ). For example, Lim, Yoo, Rho, & Ryu ( 2022 ) found that students lacking a sense of school belonging are more likely to engage in dropout behaviors. Consequently, students may experience problems related to learning interruption and academic procrastination. This is especially true in pandemic situations, where prolonged isolation and lockdowns diminish the sense of school belonging, resulting in academic disengagement and procrastination (Morán-Soto et al., 2022 ). Overall, the literature suggests an existing relationship between a sense of school belonging and academic procrastination. Students lacking this sense of school belonging are more likely to exhibit higher levels of academic procrastination and engage in dropout behaviors, and the pandemic has further aggravated this phenomenon. Therefore, this study proposes:

H8: Sense of school belonging negatively predicts academic procrastination.

To examine the above hypothesis, this study employed a decision tree model to predict academic procrastination behavior among college students. The strength of this method lies in decision trees’ capability to manage large amounts of data and identify various predictive factors and their interactions (Charbuty & Abdulazeez, 2021 ), which is important for understanding complex academic procrastination behaviors (Yang et al., 2020 ). Moreover, the decision tree prediction process is easy to understand and can be visualized by showing every decision step from the root node to the leaf nodes (Streeb et al., 2022 ). This approach enables intuitive display of the relationship patterns between predictors and the outcome variable (Yang et al., 2020 ). This study intended to utilize a decision tree model to clearly demonstrate the factors and pathways influencing academic procrastination, thereby providing insights for developing strategies and interventions to reduce academic procrastination. In short, the decision tree is an appropriate approach to adopt in this study as it is an intuitive, easy-to-understand, and effective method for prediction that enables the identification of key influencing factors. Hypothetical predictors of academic procrastination in this study are shown in Fig. 1 .

figure 1

The left panel lists hypothesized predictors of academic procrastination. The arrow symbolizes the predictive relationship. The right panel represents the predicted variable, academic procrastination, which is categorized into high and low level.

Research Method

Participants and procedures.

The location of this study was the Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region of China, chosen for its proactive educational policies and practices during the COVID-19 pandemic. For instance, the region implemented strict academic measures by issuing dropout notices to students with severe academic procrastination or overdue completion. This study adopted a three-stage random sampling method to ensure the representativeness of the data collected from September 7th to October 15th, 2022. At the initial stage, because of the stringent lockdown policies and school management regulations during the pandemic, a simple random sampling method was employed to select 3 universities out of 83 in the region by utilizing a random number generator (Calculator.net, 2022 ). However, only one university was authorized to conduct the survey. It is noteworthy that due to the pandemic, the freshmen had not started their courses, and most of the seniors were engaged in off-campus internships. Therefore, this study focused its sampling on sophomore students, covering 46 different majors, with a total of 15,000 students. Subsequently, this study utilized the Online Random Number Generator website (Calculator.net, 2022 ), randomly selected 800 sophomore students. After data collection was completed, 24 questionnaires were deemed invalid due to poor response quality (such as arbitrary ticking, consecutive repetitive answers, etc.). Ultimately, 776 valid questionnaires were obtained, including 219 males (28.2%) and 557 females (71.8%), with ages ranging from 19 to 25 years.

This study utilized an online questionnaire to collect data. Because of lockdown policies during the COVID-19 pandemic, researchers were not allowed to enter the campus. Therefore, a trained research assistant, who also serves as a university counselor of the selected university, assisted the researchers in data collection. The questionnaire was expected to take approximately 20 minutes to complete. Prior to the survey, the research assistant provided detailed information about the purpose of the study, confidentiality, and the right to withdraw to ensure voluntariness.

Additionally, participants were required to read and sign an informed consent form, affirming their understanding and agreement to participate under the terms outlined. Afterward, the trained research assistants displayed QR codes for the questionnaire to students. Those who agreed to participate in the study were required to scan the QR codes, access the survey page, answer the questions, and then submit upon completion. Additionally, the trained research assistant was prepared to assist and answer any questions from participants if there was any confusion related to the questionnaire during the process.

The online questionnaire employed in this study consists of two parts: demographic information and scales, a total of 131 items. The demographic information includes gender, age, and academic achievement. The scales section comprises seven different scales to assess the predictors and academic procrastination in this study: subjective well-being, smartphone addiction, negative emotions, self-esteem, life autonomy, pro-environmental behavior, and sense of school belonging.

Subjective well-being scale

Subjective Well-Being (SWB), as coined by Diener ( 1984 ), refers to a personal overall assessment of quality of life that encompasses two dimensions: affective and cognitive. Xing ( 2002 ) modified this scale into a Chinese version, including 20 items. It is one of the most recognized and commonly used scales to measure well-being in China. This study adopted the Chinese version of SWB, which comprises 20 items similar to the original scale. The scale utilizing a 5-point Likert scale to measure well-being ranging from “1” as strongly disagree; “2” as disagree; “3” as somewhat disagree; “4” as agree; and “5” as strongly agree. In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for the Subjective Well-Being Scale was 0.860.

Smartphone addiction scale

The Short version of the Smartphone Addiction Scale (SAS-SV) was developed by Kwon, Kim, Cho, and Yang ( 2013 ). It consists of 10 items to assess the degree of smartphone addiction. The items describe daily-life disturbance, positive anticipation, withdrawal, cyberspace-oriented relationships, overuse, and tolerance. This scale is rated on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from “1” for strongly disagree to “5” for strongly agree. Higher scores indicate higher levels of smartphone addiction. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for SAS-SV found in this study was 0.840.

Depression, anxiety, and stress scale

The Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale (DASS-42) was developed by Lovibond, Lovibond ( 1995 ) based on the three-factor model of depression, anxiety, and stress. Then, DASS-42 was later revised into a shorter version known as DASS-21 by Antony et al., ( 1998 ) to measure the levels of depression, anxiety, and stress. This scale is simple, easy to use, novel unique, and fast to operate. It has been translated into many languages for research and application in countries around the world. This scale consists of 21 items and rated on 5-point Likert scale ranging from “1” for strongly disagree to “5” for strongly agree. Higher scores indicate higher levels of depression, anxiety, and stress. In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for the scale was 0.965.

Rosenberg self-esteem scale

This study adopted the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSE), which was developed by Rosenberg ( 1965 ) to evaluate general feelings of self-worth and self-acceptance. The scale consists of 10 items, and respondents report whether the items accurately describe themselves. This scale has been widely used, it is concise and easy to score and can help participants directly assess their own positive or negative feelings. All items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from “1” for strongly disagree to “5” for strongly agree. In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for the self-esteem scale was 0.714.

Life autonomy scale

This study adapted Life Autonomy Scale from Pan and Xie ( 2010 ) to measure students’ life autonomy. The original scale consists of 70 items, including six sub-scales: ideal, life autonomy, existence, love and care, life experience, and attitude toward death. To assess the degree of life autonomy of participants, this study only selected the sub-scale ‘life autonomy,’ which includes 12 items. All items were rated on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from “1” for strongly disagree to “5” for strongly agree. Items 7 to 12 employ reverse scoring, a higher score indicates a lower life autonomy, while a lower score may suggest a higher life autonomy. In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of the scale was 0.946.

Pro-environmental behavior scale

The Pro-environmental Behavior Scale utilized in this study was developed by Liu and Wu ( 2013 ). This scale consists of 11 items across public and private dimensions. Six items pertain to behaviors in the public domain, while five items concern behaviors in the private domain. In the public domain, behaviors mainly involve participation in environmental conservation activities within public organizations, such as donating to environmental NGOs (Non-Governmental Organizations) or conservation societies. Conversely, private domain behaviors refer to environmentally friendly actions in individuals’ daily lives, such as purchasing eco-friendly products. This scale was rated on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from “1” for strongly agree to “5” for strongly disagree. Higher scores indicate higher levels of pro-environmental behavior. The scale in this study yielded a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of 0.953.

Psychological sense of school membership scale

This study employed the Psychological Sense of School Membership (PSSM) scale to assess the sense of school belonging of respondents. The original scale was developed by Goodenow ( 3.0.CO;2-X " href="/articles/s41599-024-03300-1#ref-CR39" id="ref-link-section-d139715408e993">1993 ) and consists of 18 items. It has been widely translated into multiple languages, including a Chinese version. This study adopted the Chinese version of PSSM, revised by Pan et al. ( 2011 ), which also comprises 18 items and assesses a student’s commitment to the school in terms of attachment, behavioral attitudes, and identification with the school. This scale is rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “1” for strongly agree to “5” for strongly disagree. The scale used in this study yielded a Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of 0.838.

Academic procrastination scale

This study adopted the Academic Procrastination Scale developed by Tuckman ( 1991 ) to measure academic procrastination behavior. According to Tuckman, one of the strengths of this scale is its flexibility for allowing respondents to report their own behavior, and its specific usefulness in identifying academic procrastination behavior. Its rating scale was also converted from the original four points to five points before administration to maintain consistency with the other scales. The scale consists of 16 items and is rated on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from “1” for strongly disagree to “5” for strongly agree. Higher scores indicate higher levels of academics. In this study, the reported Cronbach’s alpha coefficient of the scale was 0.920.

Statistical analyses

This study employed SPSS 26.0 for descriptive statistical analysis and Modeler 18.0 for decision tree model analysis. Descriptive statistics were used to analyze frequency distributions and trend changes in the observed data. Subsequently, the decision tree model was constructed using the C5.0 algorithm, an extension of the ID3 and the C4.5 algorithms proposed by Quinlan ( 1987 , 1996 ) and Witten, Frank, and Hall( 2005 ). This algorithm is not only suitable for large datasets but also offers faster computational speed and stronger predictive capabilities (Xiong, 2011 ). It was utilized to investigate which variables can predict the occurrence of academic procrastination.

Data coding

This study categorizes samples into two levels of academic procrastination: high and low. The questionnaire utilizes a Likert 5-point scale for scoring, with the study selecting 60% as the cutoff point. Consequently, scores of 3 or below are coded as 0, while scores above 3 are coded as 1. Based on this principle, the present study encodes the key variables predicting academic procrastination. (see Table 1 ).

The construction of decision tree

The construction of the decision tree model requires classifying samples based on the information entropy of the input dataset. Information entropy reflects the complexity within the samples. The greater the impurity within the samples (degree of impurity of a dataset), the larger the value of the information entropy, defined based on Mitchell ( 1997 ) as:

D is the training dataset with sample size m, and P k is the probability of each class of samples.

The Gain Ratio is used to measure the difference in information entropy of datasets under different classification methods. If this study chooses variable C to divide the dataset D into n subsets, then based on Quinlan ( 1996 ), the Gain Ratio is defined as:

The C5.0 algorithm selects the attribute with the maximum Gain Ratio as the splitting point, establishing several branches based on the values of this attribute, and obtaining some subsets. This selection process is repeated until the final subsets contain only data of the same category, to perform the induction classification of data (Che et al., 2011 ).

Pruning of the decision tree

Based on the decision tree model constructed from the training samples, the dataset is recursively traversed to each leaf node. Specifically, the C5.0 algorithm employs post-pruning to systematically prune the leaf nodes, layer by layer. The mean square error of the dataset nodes is calculated. If the mean square error decreases after pruning, the node is removed; otherwise, it is retained (Quinlan, 1998 ).

Evaluation of the decision tree

In this study, as recommended by Gholamy, Kreinovich, and Kosheleva ( 2018 ), 70% of the sample data ( n  = 544) is selected as the training data, while 30% of the sample data ( n  = 232) is used as the testing data. The testing data reflects the extent to which the model constructed from the training data is suitable to new data. Accuracy, precision, and recall are indicators used to evaluate the quality of the model (Han et al., 2019 ). Accuracy refers to the proportion of correctly classified samples out of all samples. Precision refers to the proportion of true positive samples among the predicted positive samples. Recall refers to the proportion of true positive samples correctly predicted out of all actual positive samples. Specifically, recall is calculated as TP (true positive) divided by TP (true positive) plus FN (false negative), and accuracy is calculated as TP (true positive) divided by TP (true positive) plus FP (false positive).

Descriptive statistics

The descriptive statistics are summarized in Table 2 . The forecast target, students’ academic procrastination behavior, shows a good status. The mean value of academic procrastination was 2.593 (with a standard deviation of 0.635), which is lower than 60% of the full score. This means that no more than half of the students were in a state of high academic procrastination. We then encoded each variable by assigning a value of 1 to cases with scores above 60% of the full score and a value of 0 to all other cases.

Prediction analysis of academic procrastination

As presented in Fig. 2 below, the predictive factors of academic procrastination include subjective well-being, smartphone addiction, negative emotions, self-esteem, life autonomy, pro-environment behavior, academic achievement, and sense of school belonging. Additionally, respondents with low academic procrastination accounted for 79.963%. The root node is the topmost node in a decision tree model, and the branches below this top node represent the outcomes of decisions (see Fig. 2 ). The closer a predictive variable is to the root node, the higher its importance, indicating the degree of importance of the predictive variable.

figure 2

The gray rectangle represents a node. The value inside a node indicates the quantity and distribution of samples. Blue and red squares represent the volume and proportion of samples within the node. The value ‘n’ denotes the number of samples in the node. The ‘%‘ value indicates the percentage of samples in the node relative to the total number of samples. ‘Total’ represents the cumulative total number of samples in the node.

The importance of predictor variables can be inferred from Fig. 3 . Subjective well-being is the most important predictor variable to academic procrastination, followed by smartphone addiction and negative emotions in second and third place. Self-esteem, autonomy, and prosocial behavior rank fourth, fifth, and sixth in importance, while academic achievement and school belonging have the least importance.

figure 3

The horizontal axis represents the importance of the predictive impact, while the vertical axis lists the predictors. The blue bars indicate the importance of each predictor on academic procrastination, with longer bars representing more important prediction.

Model evaluation

Tables 3 and 4 respectively present the confusion matrix and classification accuracy of the research model. The accuracy of training samples in the model is 87.50%, and the accuracy of test samples is 85.78%. As reported in Table 5 , for the test samples, the model’s precision rate for predicting low academic procrastination is 90.24%, with a recall rate of 94.12%.

This study utilized a decision tree model and the C5.0 algorithm to construct an eight-factor model for predicting academic procrastination. Additionally, the study ranked the importance of predictors to academic procrastination based on their contribution. The discussions of the findings of this study are based on the order of importance of predictive factors for academic procrastination, which are subjective well-being, smartphone addiction, negative emotion, self-esteem, life autonomy, pro-environmental behavior, academic achievement, and sense of school belonging.

The findings of this study suggesting the three most important predictors of academic procrastination were subjective well-being, smartphone addiction, and negative emotion. The reason may lie in the fact that students with higher levels of subjective well-being are more likely to possess conscientious and open positive personality traits (Abdullahi et al., 2020 ). These students also possess a heightened sense of time value and time monitoring, thereby avoiding academic procrastination behaviors in comparison to their counterparts who have lower levels of subjective well-being (Berber Çelik & Odaci, 2022 ). Moreover, higher levels of subjective well-being can assist students in adapting throughout the learning process and tackling academic tasks (Ran et al., 2023 ). Conversely, lower levels of subjective well-being tend to exhibit negative personality traits such as Neuroticism, which increases the likelihood of academic procrastination (Abdullahi et al., 2020 ).

Students with smartphone addiction often exhibit a lack of attention and self-control (Geng et al., 2021 ). This addiction can easily lead to a tendency to use smartphones to escape study pressure, thus increasing the likelihood of academic procrastination behaviors (Troll et al., 2021 ). This phenomenon is particularly pronounced in China, as the country has the highest number of smartphone users in the world. Smartphone addiction intensified even further as students became increasingly dependent on smartphones for online learning during pandemic (Albursan et al., 2022 ). However, heavy screen time can also easily lead to the misuse and overuse of smartphones, such as spending excessive time on social media, entertainment, or for gaming purposes. Smartphone addiction can deteriorate learning efficiency and self-control, while increasing procrastination behaviors (Troll et al., 2021 ).

Research indicates that negative emotions can predict procrastination behavior. Neuroimaging studies show that negative emotions activate specific brain regions in the anterior insula and amygdala. These regions are associated with procrastination behaviors (Barrett & Satpute, 2013 ; Seeley et al., 2007 ). Evidence suggests that the stronger the negative emotions, the more likely procrastination behaviors are to manifest (Wang et al., 2022 ). A study conducted by Rahimi and Vallerand ( 2021 ) during COVID-19 also reveals that negative emotions such as fear, anxiety, and depression are associated with academic procrastination. These findings from past literature could explain how prolonged lockdowns and high academic pressure are likely to diminish students’ goal-setting, self-monitoring, and self-regulation, leading to a significant increase in negative emotions among college students. This, in turn, further contributes to the occurrence of academic procrastination behaviors.

Khurshid, Batool ( 2018 ) considered academic procrastination as the product of low self-esteem, and they argued that self-esteem can negatively predict academic procrastination. In times of crisis, college students with higher self-esteem often find it easier to immerse themselves in academic life and are more likely to overcome academic procrastination (Yang et al., 2021 ). Additionally, Brando-Garrido et al. ( 2020 ) pointed out that self-esteem not only affects academic confidence and motivation but also indirectly influences academic procrastination behavior through various aspects, including emotional regulation and proficiency in goal establishment.

Past literature suggests life autonomy can negatively predict academic procrastination, students with higher life autonomy are more self-directed and have stronger self-control, thus less likely to engage in academic procrastination behavior (Codina et al., 2018 ). This is because students with higher life autonomy are also likely to possess greater self-regulation and take proactive action to avoid the occurrence of academic procrastination behavior. This viewpoint has received support from neuroscience studies, where non-invasive technologies such as Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (TDCS) and Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) were used to stimulate the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) to enhance participants’ self-control and thereby reduce procrastination behavior (Feng, Wang, & Su, 2021 ).

Ateş ( 2020 ) proposes that pro-environmental behavior is an important predictor for academic procrastination given pro-environmental behavior often requires strong planning skills. Empirical evidence suggests that those who actively engage in pro-environmental behavior also likely demonstrate stronger proficiency in goal-setting and execution of plans, thereby mitigating procrastination behavior (Sawitri et al., 2015 ; Yuriev et al., 2020 ). Additionally, pro-environmental behavior also reflects personal self-regulation and self-discipline, both of which are crucial for reducing procrastination behavior (Akinci, 2021 ; Colombo et al., 2023 ). The findings of this study suggest that even in times of crisis, understanding and promoting pro-environmental behavior is important not only for promoting environmental protection but also for improving academic performance and reducing procrastination behavior.

In comparison to the other predictors mentioned above, the findings of this study indicate that the impact of academic achievement and a sense of school belonging on academic procrastination are relatively small. Although previous studies supported the association between these two predictors and academic procrastination (Lim et al., 2022 ; Morán-Soto et al., 2022 ), during times of crisis, students may prioritize health, family, and economic situation (Tadesse & Muluye, 2020 ; Verma & Prakash, 2020 ). Under such circumstances, academic achievement may no longer be the top priority for students, but rather addressing the various issues brought about by the crisis (Brion & Kiral, 2021 ; Hartshorn & Benjamin, 2020 ).

Lastly, the weakest predictor for academic procrastination found in this study is the sense of school belonging. One possible explanation could be the impact of long-term online learning during the epidemic, which weakens the connection between students’ sense of school belonging, and their tendency to procrastinate academically. Literature suggests that a sense of school belonging often has a role to play in academic procrastination through other mediators. For example, Dong and Izadpanah ( 2022 ) argue that the sense of school belonging can indirectly affect academic procrastination by influencing self-efficacy and emotions. Furthermore, a sense of school belonging can also reduce the degree of academic procrastination by alleviating psychological pressures such as anxiety and tension (Abdollahi et al., 2020 ). Despite academic achievement and a sense of school belonging are not the most important predictors for academic procrastination in this study, they are still important for students’ overall academic performance and psychological well-being during public crises. Therefore, these predictors should not be disregarded.

This study employs a decision tree model to predict academic procrastination. The results show that the model has an accuracy of 85.78%, indicating its effectiveness in predicting academic procrastination. The findings of this study suggest that subjective well-being, smartphone addiction, and negative emotions are core predictors of academic procrastination. In particular, subjective well-being, the most significant predictor, underscores the crucial role of psychological well-being and life satisfaction in academic behavior. Moreover, smartphone addiction and negative emotions reveal the significant impact of modern technology dependence and emotional health on academic procrastination. It is worth noting that factors such as self-esteem, life autonomy, pro-environmental behavior, academic achievement, and sense of school belonging, although having a relatively small predictive effect on academic procrastination in this study, should not be disregarded. Instead, they remain important considerations for understanding the complexity of academic procrastination behavior. These factors provide insights for mitigating academic procrastination by promoting personal well-being, enhancing self-regulation, and strengthening school belonging. The findings of this study not only offer new perspectives on understanding and predicting academic procrastination but also provide empirical evidence for developing effective interventions to address it. In conclusion, this study makes a significant contribution to the understanding of academic procrastination behavior, both in times of crisis and beyond, and offers practical guidance for effectively tackling academic procrastination among college education students.

Implication

This study has enriched the applicability of the Biopsychosocial Model in the context of public crises by suggesting that the predictors associated with academic procrastination are indeed interrelated with students’ biological, psychological, and social aspects. Simultaneously, this study could provide a series of practical implications for educational stakeholders. The findings of the study suggest that students bear the primary responsibility for combating academic procrastination, but maintaining personal well-being is essential for improving self-regulation, which is necessary for effectively addressing procrastination. This would enable students to better utilize campus resources, such as engaging in group studies and utilizing counseling services to mitigate tendencies toward academic procrastination. Additionally, it is important for lecturers to provide academic and emotional support to enhance students’ self-esteem and autonomy as they are significant factors in addressing academic procrastination behavior. Lastly, the findings of the study could offer insights to university management on optimizing student support services such as wellness centers, counseling services, and academic advising centers. This optimization would aim to consistently ensure student well-being and enhance the sense of school belonging, especially during times of crisis to reduce academic procrastination behavior among students.

Limitation and future research

This study has certain limitations. Adopting a cross-sectional design may restrict the depth of understanding over time. Moreover, the respondents all come from a university in the Guangxi Zhuang Autonomous Region, which could potentially limit the generalizability of the research results. Future research could focus on enhancing understanding by observing the interactions between the predictors and changes in academic procrastination over time. Additionally, recruiting participants from different regions or institutions could improve the generalizability of the findings. Researcher is also encouraged to investigate other potential variables that are not included in this study to expand the understanding of academic procrastination behavior and its determinants. This could be beneficial in the endeavor to reduce academic procrastination behavior beyond the pandemic.

Data availability

The datasets generated during and analyzed during the current study are not publicly available due to institutional confidentiality regulations but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abdi Zarrin S, Gracia E (2020) Prediction of academic procrastination by fear of failure and self-regulation. Educ. Sci.: Theory Pract. 20(3):34–43

Google Scholar  

Abdollahi A, Maleki Farab N, Panahipour S, Allen KA (2020) Academic hardiness as a moderator between evaluative concerns perfectionism and academic procrastination in students. J. Genet. Psychol. 181(5):365–374. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.2020.1783194

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Abdullahi AM, Orji R, Rabiu AM, Kawu AA (2020) Personality and subjective well-being: towards personalized persuasive interventions for health and well-being. Online J. Public Health Inform. 12(1):e1. https://doi.org/10.5210/ojphi.v12i1.10335

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Akinci T (2021) Determination of predictive relationships between problematic smartphone use, Self-regulation, academic procrastination and academic stress through modelling. Int. J. Progressive Educ. 17(1):35–53

Article   Google Scholar  

Albujar Moreno WA, Castro Portillas LE (2020) Procrastinación académica y niveles de ansiedad en estudiantes de secundaria de una Institución Educativa de Lambayeque , 2019 . http://repositorio.unprg.edu.pe/handle/20.500.12893/8977

Albursan IS, Al. Qudah MF, Al-Barashdi HS, Bakhiet SF, Darandari E, Al-Asqah SS, Hammad HI, Al-Khadher MM, Qara S, Al-Mutairy SH, Albursan HI (2022) Smartphone addiction among university students in light of the COVID-19 pandemic: prevalence, relationship to academic procrastination, quality of life, gender and educational stage. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19(16):16. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph191610439

Antony MM, Bieling PJ, Cox BJ, Enns MW, Swinson RP (1998) Psychometric properties of the 42-item and 21-item versions of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales in clinical groups and a community sample. Psychol. Assess. 10(2):176

Arifiana IY, Rahmawati H, Hanurawan F, Eva N (2020) Stop academic procrastination during COVID-19: Academic procrastination reduces subjective well-being. KnE Soc. Sci. 4(15):312–325. https://doi.org/10.18502/kss.v4i15.8220

Ateş H (2020) Merging theory of planned behavior and value identity personal norm model to explain pro-environmental behaviors. Sustain. Prod. Consum. 24:169–180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.spc.2020.07.006

Barrett LF, Satpute AB (2013) Large-scale brain networks in affective and social neuroscience: Towards an integrative functional architecture of the brain. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23(3):361–372. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.012

Article   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Batool SS (2020) Academic achievement: Interplay of positive parenting, self‐esteem, and academic procrastination. Aust. J. Psychol. 72(2):174–187. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajpy.12280

Berber Çelik Ç, Odaci H (2022) Subjective well-being in university students: What are the impacts of procrastination and attachment styles? Br. J. Guid. Counsel. 50(5):768–781. https://doi.org/10.1080/03069885.2020.1803211

Bolbolian M, Asgari S, Sefidi F, Zadeh AS (2021) The relationship between test anxiety and academic procrastination among the dental students. J. Educ. Health Promotion, 10. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8057172/

Brando-Garrido C, Montes-Hidalgo J, Limonero JT, Gómez-Romero MJ, Tomás-Sábado J (2020) Relationship of academic procrastination with perceived competence, coping, self-esteem and self-efficacy in Nursing students. Enfermería Clínica (Engl. Ed.) 30(6):398–403. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.enfcle.2019.07.013

Brion C, Kiral B (2021) COVID-19 crisis challenges and management strategies of educational leaders in America. Int. J. Contemp. Educ. Res. 8(4):170–183

Bu X, Wu L, Wang H (2021) Impact of college students’ academic procrastination on subjective well-being. Soc. Behav. Pers: an Int. J 49(7):1–13

Cai Y (2017) Study on the status and countermeasures of academic procrastination among high school students [Master's thesis, Inner Mongolia Normal University]. CNKI. DOI: CNKI:CDMD:2.1017.092732

Calculator.net (2022) Random Number Generator. Retrieved 26 August, 2022, from https://www.calculator.net/random-number-generator.html

Charbuty B, Abdulazeez A (2021) Classification based on decision tree algorithm for machine learning. J. Appl. Sci. Technol. Trends 2(01):01. https://doi.org/10.38094/jastt20165

Che D, Liu Q, Rasheed K, Tao X (2011) Decision Tree and ensemble learning algorithms with their applications in bioinformatics. In H. R. Arabnia & Q.-N. Tran (Eds.), Software Tools and Algorithms for Biological Systems (pp. 191–199). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-7046-6_19

Chen B, Liu F, Ding S, Ying X, Wang L, Wen Y (2017) Gender differences in factors associated with smartphone addiction: A cross-sectional study among medical college students. BMC Psychiatry 17(1):341. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-017-1503-z

Ching KH, Tak LM (2017) The structural model in parenting style, attachment style, self-regulation and self-esteem for smartphone addiction. IAFOR J. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 3(1):85–103

MathSciNet   Google Scholar  

Codina N, Valenzuela R, Pestana JV, Gonzalez-Conde, J (2018) Relations between student procrastination and teaching styles: autonomy-supportive and controlling. Front. Psychol., 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00809

Colombo SL, Chiarella SG, Lefrançois C, Fradin J, Raffone A, Simione L (2023) Why knowing about climate change is not enough to change: a perspective paper on the factors explaining the environmental knowledge-action gap. Sustainability 15(20):20. https://doi.org/10.3390/su152014859

Cui G, Yin Y, Li S, Chen L, Liu X, Tang K, Li Y (2021) Longitudinal relationships among problematic mobile phone use, bedtime procrastination, sleep quality and depressive symptoms in Chinese college students: A cross-lagged panel analysis. BMC Psychiatry 21(1):449. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-021-03451-4

Deng Y, Ye B, Yang Q (2022) COVID-19 related emotional stress and bedtime procrastination among college students in China: A moderated mediation model. Nat. Sci. Sleep. ume 14:1437–1447. https://doi.org/10.2147/NSS.S371292

Diener E (1984) Subjective well-being. Psychol. Bull. 95(3):542–575. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.542

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Dong Y, Izadpanah S (2022) The effect of corrective feedback from female teachers on formative assessments: Educational resilience, educational belongingness, and academic procrastination in an English language course. Curr. Psychol 42(33):29734. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-03825-1

Engel GL (1981) The clinical application of the biopsychosocial model. J. Med. Philos. 6(2):101–124

Feng F (2018) Investigation on the status of depression and anxiety among medical college students# br. J. Hebei Med. Univ. 38(6):636

Feng T, Wang X, Su T (2021) The developmental cognitive mechanisms and neural basis of procrastination behavior. Adv in Psychol Sci. 29(4):586–596

Ferrari JR, Camilleri A (2021) When you have a procrastinating client or colleague, time management will not work. Crim. Justice 36:27

Foa R, Gilbert S, Fabian MO (2020) COVID-19 and subjective well-being: Separating the effects of lockdowns from the pandemic. Available at SSRN 3674080 . https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=3674080

Gamage KAA, Wijesuriya DI, Ekanayake SY, Rennie AEW, Lambert CG, Gunawardhana N (2020) Online delivery of teaching and laboratory practices: continuity of University programmes during COVID-19 pandemic. Educ. Sci. 10(10):10. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci10100291 . Article

Gao L, Zhang J, Xie H, Nie Y, Zhao Q, Zhou Z (2020) Effect of the mobile phone-related background on inhibitory control of problematic mobile phone use: An event-related potentials study. Addict. Behav. 108:106363

García-Ros R, Pérez-González F, Tomás JM, Sancho P (2023) Effects of self-regulated learning and procrastination on academic stress, subjective well-being, and academic achievement in secondary education. Curr. Psychol. 42(30):26602–26616. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-022-03759-8

Geng Y, Gu J, Wang J, Zhang R (2021) Smartphone addiction and depression, anxiety: The role of bedtime procrastination and self-control. J. Affect. Disord. 293:415–421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2021.06.062

Gholamy A, Kreinovich V, Kosheleva O (2018) Why 70/30 or 80/20 relation between training and testing sets: A pedagogical explanation. Int. J. Intell. Technol. Appl. Stat. 11(2):105–111. https://doi.org/10.6148/IJITAS.201806_11(2).0003

Goodenow C (1993) The psychological sense of school membership among adolescents: Scale development and educational correlates. Psychol. Sch. 30(1):79–90. 3.0.CO;2-X" data-track="click||click_references" data-track-action="external reference" data-track-value="external reference" data-track-label="10.1002/1520-6807(199301)30:1 3.0.CO;2-X">https://doi.org/10.1002/1520-6807(199301)30:1<79::AID-PITS2310300113>3.0.CO;2-X

3.0.CO;2-X" data-track-item_id="10.1002/1520-6807(199301)30:1 3.0.CO;2-X" data-track-value="article reference" data-track-action="article reference" href="https://doi.org/10.1002%2F1520-6807%28199301%2930%3A1%3C79%3A%3AAID-PITS2310300113%3E3.0.CO%3B2-X" aria-label="Article reference 39" data-doi="10.1002/1520-6807(199301)30:1 3.0.CO;2-X">Article   Google Scholar  

Gopalan M, Linden-Carmichael A, Lanza S (2022) College students’ sense of belonging and mental health amidst the COVID-19 pandemic. J. Adolesc. Health 70(2):228–233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2021.10.010

Goroshit M, Hen M (2021) Academic procrastination and academic performance: Do learning disabilities matter? Curr. Psychol. 40(5):2490–2498. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00183-3

Guay F (2022) Applying self-determination theory to education: regulations types, psychological needs, and autonomy supporting behaviors. Can. J. Sch. Psychol. 37(1):75–92. https://doi.org/10.1177/08295735211055355

Gutiérrez-García AG, Huerta-Cortés M, Landeros-Velazquez MG (2020) Academic procrastination in study habits and its relationship with self-reported executive functions in high school students. J. Psychol. Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.47485/2693-2490.1004

Hamvai C, Kiss H, Vörös H, Fitzpatrick KM, Vargha A, Pikó BF (2023) Association between impulsivity and cognitive capacity decrease is mediated by smartphone addiction, academic procrastination, bedtime procrastination, sleep insufficiency and daytime fatigue among medical students: A path analysis. BMC Med. Educ. 23(1):537. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12909-023-04522-8

Han J, Fang M, Ye S, Chen C, Wan Q, Qian X (2019) Using decision tree to predict response rates of consumer satisfaction, attitude, and loyalty surveys. Sustainability 11(8):8. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11082306

Hartshorn KJ, Benjamin LM (2020) The effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on ESL learners and TESOL practitioners in the United States. Int. J. TESOL Stud. 2(2):140–156. https://doi.org/10.46451/ijts.2020.09.11

Horani LF, Dong L (2023) Understanding sustainable purchase intention of smartphone users Interface: Evidence from China. Front. Psychol. 14:1122801

Jiao H, Guo J (2020) The impact of life meaning on academic procrastination and depression among college students: A moderated mediation effect. J. Jinggangshan Univ. (Nat. Sci.) 41(1):102–106

Karataş H (2015) Correlation among academic procrastination, personality traits, and academic achievement. Anthropologist 20:243–255

Katsumata S, Ichikohji T, Nakano S, Yamaguchi S, Ikuine F (2022) Changes in the use of mobile devices during the crisis: Immediate response to the COVID-19 pandemic. Comput. Hum. Behav. Rep. 5:100168

Khurshid S, Batool S, Jahangir H (2018) Academic Procrastination as a Product of Low Self-Esteem: A Mediational Role of Academic Self-Efficacy. Pak. J. Psychol. Res, 32(1), 195. https://pjpr.scione.com/cms/abstract.php?id=232

Klingsieck KB (2013) Procrastination. Eur. Psychol. 18(1):24–34. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000138

Kuhfeld M, Soland J, Tarasawa B, Johnson A, Ruzek E, Liu J (2020) Projecting the potential impact of COVID-19 school closures on academic achievement. Educ. Res. 49(8):549–565. https://doi.org/10.3102/0013189X20965918

Kurtovic A, Vrdoljak G, Idzanovic A (2019) Predicting procrastination: The role of academic achievement, self-efficacy and perfectionism. Int. J. Educ. Psychol.: IJEP 8(1):1–26

Kwon M, Kim D-J, Cho H, Yang S (2013) The smartphone addiction scale: development and validation of a short version for adolescents. PLOS ONE 8(12):e83558. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0083558

Article   ADS   CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Lillemo SC (2014) Measuring the effect of procrastination and environmental awareness on households’ energy-saving behaviours: An empirical approach. Energy Policy 66:249–256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.enpol.2013.10.077

Lim HJ, Yoo JE, Rho M, Ryu JJ (2022) Exploration of variables predicting sense of school belonging using the machine learning method—Group Mnet. Psychol. Rep. , 00332941221133005. https://doi.org/10.1177/00332941221133005

Limone P, Sinatra M, Ceglie F, Monacis L (2020) Examining procrastination among university students through the lens of the self-regulated learning model. Behav. Sci. 10(12):184

Liu F, Xu Y, Yang T, Li Z, Dong Y, Chen L, Sun X (2022) The mediating roles of time management and learning strategic approach in the relationship between smartphone addiction and academic procrastination. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. ume 15:2639–2648. https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S373095

Liu X, Wu J (2013) College students’ environmental values and pro-environmental behaviors: The mediating role of environmental concern. Stud. of Psychol. and Behav. 11(6):780–785

Lovibond PF, Lovibond SH (1995) The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the beck depression and anxiety inventories. Behav. Res. Ther. 33(3):335–343. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(94)00075-U

Maxwell BE (1992) Hostility, depression, and self-esteem among troubled and homeless adolescents in crisis. J. Youth Adoles. 21(2):139–150. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01537333

Article   CAS   Google Scholar  

Meng Y (2020) Mental health status of college students under regular prevention and control of coronavirus disease 2019 epidemic. Acad. J. Second Milit. Med. Univ. 41(12):958–965

Mi L, Zhao J, Xu T, Yang H, Lv T, Shang K, Qiao Y, Zhang Z (2021) How does COVID-19 emergency cognition influence public pro-environmental behavioral intentions? An affective event perspective. Resour., Conserv. Recycl 168:105467. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.resconrec.2021.105467

Mitchell TM (1997) Does machine learning really work? AI Mag. 18(3):11–11

Morán-Soto G, Marsh A, González Peña OI, Sheppard M, Gómez-Quiñones JI, Benson LC (2022) Effect of the COVID-19 pandemic on the sense of belonging in higher education for STEM students in the United States and Mexico. Sustainability 14(24):24. https://doi.org/10.3390/su142416627

Morosanova VI, Fomina TG, Bondarenko IN (2021) The dynamics of the interrelationships between conscious self-regulation, psychological well-being and school-related subjective well-being in adolescents: A three-year cross-lagged panel study. Psychol. Russ. 14(3):34

Nwosu KC, Ikwuka DO, Onyinyechi MU, Unachukwu GC (2020) Does the association of social media use with problematic internet behaviours predict undergraduate students’ academic procrastination? Can. J. Learn. Technol. 46(1):n1

Pan C-Y, Xie M-Y (2010) Development of the life attitude scale. Tzu Chi University. J Educ. Res. 6:185–229

Pan F, Wang Q, Song L, Ding J, Dai J (2011) Reliability and validity test of the Chinese version of the School Belongingness Scale. Chinese J of Clin. Psychol. 19(2):3

Park S (2019) Identification of overall innovation behavior by using a decision tree: The case of a Korean manufacturer. Sustainability 11(22):22. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11226207

Peixoto EM, Pallini AC, Vallerand RJ, Rahimi S, Silva MV (2021) The role of passion for studies on academic procrastination and mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 24(3):877–893. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-021-09636-9

Podlesek A, Komidar L, Kavcic V (2021) The relationship between perceived stress and subjective cognitive decline during the COVID-19 epidemic. Front. Psychol. 12:647971. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.647971

Quinlan JR (1987) Simplifying decision trees. Int. J. Man-Mach. Stud. 27(3):221–234. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0020-7373(87)80053-6

Quinlan JR (1996) Improved use of continuous attributes in C4.5. J. Artif. Intell. Res. 4:77–90. https://doi.org/10.1613/jair.279

Quinlan, JR (1998). C 5.0: An informal tutorial. Htt p://Ww w . Iastud. Hiof. No/ ∼ Rolando/ML/C50tutorial. Html . https://cir.nii.ac.jp/crid/1570572699872618368

Rahimi S, Vallerand RJ (2021) The role of passion and emotions in academic procrastination during a pandemic (COVID-19). Pers. Individ. Differ 179:110852. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.110852

Ran J, Liu H, Yuan Y, Yu X, Dong T (2023) Linking career exploration, self-reflection, career calling, career adaptability and subjective well-being: a self-regulation theory perspective. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. ume 16:2805–2817. https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S420666

Ratan ZA, Parrish A-M, Zaman SB, Alotaibi MS, Hosseinzadeh H (2021) Smartphone addiction and associated health outcomes in adult populations: A systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18(22):12257

Rosenberg M (1965) Rosenberg self-esteem scale (RSE). Accept. Commit. Ther. Meas. Pack. 61(52):18

Safavian SR, Landgrebe D (1991) A survey of decision tree classifier methodology. IEEE Trans. Syst. Man, Cybern. 21(3):660–674. https://doi.org/10.1109/21.97458

Article   MathSciNet   Google Scholar  

Samson AO (2021) Understanding university undergraduates’ attribution styles in academic procrastination. AU eJournal Interdiscip. Res. (ISSN: 2408-1906) 6(1):1, http://www.assumptionjournal.au.edu/index.php/eJIR/article/view/5287

Saplavska J, Jerkunkova A (2018) Academic procrastination and anxiety among students . 17th International Scientific Conference Engineering for Rural Development. https://doi.org/10.22616/ERDev2018.17.N357

Sawitri DR, Hadiyanto H, Hadi SP (2015) Pro-environmental behavior from a socialcognitive theory perspective. Procedia Environ. Sci. 23:27–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.proenv.2015.01.005

Schouwenburg HC (1995) Academic procrastination: Theoretical notions, measurement, and research. In Procrastination and task avoidance: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 71–96). Plenum Press. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-0227-6_4

Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, Reiss AL, Greicius MD (2007) Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27(9):2349–2356. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007

Serrano Aguirre S (2020) The Impact of Natural Disasters on Academic Achievement Amongst Emerging Adults . https://hdl.handle.net/10657/6573

Sharma H, Kumar S (2016) A survey on decision tree algorithms of classification in data mining. Int. J. Sci. Res. (IJSR) 5(4):2094–2097

Shim M-S, Kim S, Choi M, Choi JY, Park CG, Kim GS (2022) Developing an app-based self-management program for people living with HIV: A randomized controlled pilot study during the COVID-19 pandemic. Sci. Rep. 12(1):19401. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-19238-w

Shu Y, Lin W, Yang J, Huang P, Li B, Zhang X (2022) How social support predicts anxiety among university students during COVID‐19 control phase: Mediating roles of self‐esteem and resilience. Anal. Soc. Issues Public Policy 22(2):490–505. https://doi.org/10.1111/asap.12314

Skrbinjek V, Dermol V (2019) Predicting students’ satisfaction using a decision tree. Tert. Educ. Manag. 25(2):101–113. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11233-018-09018-5

Sobia N, Gulfam H, Muhammad L (2021) Influence of perfectionism and academic motivation on academic procrastination among students. J. Educ. Sci. Res., Spring2021 8 Issue 1:p13–p20. 8p

Solomon LJ, Rothblum ED (1984) Academic procrastination: Frequency and cognitive-behavioral correlates. J. Counsel Psychol. 31(4):503–509. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.31.4.503

Statista (2021) The number of smartphone users by leading countries as of 2021. Available online: https://www.statista.com/statistics/748053/worldwide-top-countries-smartphone-users/

Steel P (2007) The nature of procrastination: A meta-analytic and theoretical review of quintessential self-regulatory failure. Psychol Bull. 133(1):65–94. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.65

Streeb D, Metz Y, Schlegel U, Schneider B, El-Assady M, Neth H, Chen M, Keim DA (2022) Task-based visual interactive modeling: decision trees and rule-based classifiers. IEEE Trans. Vis. Comput. Graph. 28(9):3307–3323. https://doi.org/10.1109/TVCG.2020.3045560

Svartdal F, Dahl TI, Gamst-Klaussen T, Koppenborg M, Klingsieck KB (2020) How study environments foster academic procrastination: overview and recommendations. Front. Psychol., 11 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.540910

Tadesse S, Muluye W (2020) The impact of COVID-19 pandemic on education system in developing countries: a review. Open J. Soc. Sci. 08(10):10. https://doi.org/10.4236/jss.2020.810011

Tarkar P (2020) Impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on education system. Int. J. Adv. Sci. Technol. 29:3812–3814

Tian X, Liu X, Xiao Z, Lin Q (2023) Undergraduates’ negative emotion and academic procrastination during COVID-19: Life autonomy as a mediator and sense of school belonging as a moderator. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 16:3151–3169. https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S413738

Toquero CM (2020) Challenges and opportunities for higher education amid the COVID-19 pandemic: The Philippine context. Pedagog. Res., 5 (4). https://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ1263557

Town R, Hayes D, Fonagy P, Stapley E (2022) A qualitative investigation of LGBTQ+ young people’s experiences and perceptions of self-managing their mental health. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 31(9):1441–1454. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-021-01783-w

Troll ES, Friese M, Loschelder DD (2021) How students’ self-control and smartphone-use explain their academic performance. Comput. Hum. Behav. 117:106624. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106624

Tuckman BW (1991) The development and concurrent validity of the procrastination scale. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 51(2):473–480. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164491512022

Türel YK, Dokumacı O (2022) Use of media and technology, academic procrastination, and academic achievement in adolescence. Particip. Educ. Res. 9(2):2. https://doi.org/10.17275/per.22.50.9.2

Verma AK, Prakash S (2020) Impact of COVID-19 on Environment and Society (SSRN Scholarly Paper 3644567). https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=3644567

Vlachopanou P, Karagiannopoulou E (2022) Defense styles, academic procrastination, psychological wellbeing, and approaches to learning: A person-oriented approach. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 210(3):186–193

Wang B, Liu Y, Qian J, Parker SK (2021) Achieving effective remote working during the COVID-19 pandemic: a work design perspective. Appl. Psychol. 70(1):16–59. https://doi.org/10.1111/apps.12290

Wang Y, Gao H, Liu J, Fan X (2021) Academic procrastination in college students: The role of self-leadership. Personal. Individ. Differ. 178:110866

Wang Q, Kou Z, Du Y, Wang K, Xu Y (2022) Academic procrastination and negative emotions among adolescents during the COVID-19 pandemic: the mediating and buffering effects of online-shopping addiction. Front. Psychol., 12 . https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.789505

Witten IH, Frank E, Hall MA (2005) Credibility: Evaluating what’s been learned. Data Mining: Practical Machine Learning Tools and Techniques , 143–186

Wu H, Li S, Zheng J, Guo J (2020) Medical students’ motivation and academic performance: The mediating roles of self-efficacy and learning engagement. Med. Educ. Online 25(1):1742964. https://doi.org/10.1080/10872981.2020.1742964

Wu X, Kumar V, Ross Quinlan J, Ghosh J, Yang Q, Motoda H, McLachlan GJ, Ng A, Liu B, Yu PS, Zhou Z-H, Steinbach M, Hand DJ, Steinberg D (2008) Top 10 algorithms in data mining. Knowl. Inf. Syst. 14(1):1–37. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10115-007-0114-2

Xing ZJ (2002) A review of research on the measurement of subjective well-being. Psychol. Sci. 25(3):336–338

Xiong P (2011) Data mining algorithms and Clementine practice. Beijing: Tsinghua University Press. Retrieved from https://book.douban.com/subject/6113968/

Yadav CS, Monga S, Tanwar S (2020) The effect of time management on subjective well-being among university students. Int. J. Manage. (IJM) , 11(12). https://www.academia.edu/download/67799970/IJM_11_12_150.pdf

Yang X, Liu R-D, Ding Y, Hong W, Jiang S (2021) The relations between academic procrastination and self-esteem in adolescents: A longitudinal study. Curr. Psychol. 42(9):7534–7548. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02075-x

Yang Y, Hooshyar D, Pedaste M, Wang M, Huang Y-M, Lim H (2020) Prediction of students’ procrastination behaviour through their submission behavioural pattern in online learning. J. Ambient Intell. Hum. Comput. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12652-020-02041-8

Yilmaz Ş (2023) Subjective Well-Being. Multidisciplinary perspectives in educational and social sciences VI , 87

Yu Z, Luo H, Tan W, Niu L (2022) A case study of social work leadership in the pandemic intervention in Wuhan. Br. J. Soc. Work 52(4):2183–2197. https://doi.org/10.1093/bjsw/bcab179

Yuriev A, Dahmen M, Paillé P, Boiral O, Guillaumie L (2020) Pro-environmental behaviors through the lens of the theory of planned behavior: A scoping review. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 155:104660

Zebardast L, Radaei M (2022) The influence of global crises on reshaping pro-environmental behavior, case study: The COVID-19 pandemic. Sci. Total Environ. 811:151436. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2021.151436

Zhang MX, Wu AM (2020) Effects of smartphone addiction on sleep quality among Chinese university students: The mediating role of self-regulation and bedtime procrastination. Addict. Behav. 111:106552

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Preschool and Early Education, Guiyang Preschool Education College, Guiyang, China

School of Distance Education, Universiti Sains Malaysia, Penang, Malaysia

Xiangwei Liu

Wenzhou Business College, Wenzhou, China

Liuzhou Vocational & Technical College, Liuzhou, China

Mengmeng Zhong

Guizhou Aerospace Vocational and Technical College, Guiyang, China

Qingqing Wang

Research Institute of Higher Education, Yunnan University, Kunming, China

Xiangmei Zhu

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

P.S.: Conceptualization; Supervision, Writing - review & editing. X.L. (Corresponding): Data curation, Methodology, Writing - original draft. X.C.: Methodology, Software. M.Z.: Software, Validation. Q.W.: Project administration, Resources. X.Z.: Formal analysis.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Xiangwei Liu .

Ethics declarations

Competing interests.

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

Ethical approval

This study was performed in line with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. Approval was granted by Institutional Review Board of Liuzhou Vocational and Technical College (No. LVTC-2023-06-879).

Informed consent

All participants were informed and clearly explained to the purpose of the study. They were assured of confidentiality and, anonymity before they willingly signed the informed written consent form. The questionnaires were administered to the respondents during school working hours without interfering with the school-set timetable. Participants who opted not to participate in this research even after the distribution of questionnaires were allowed to withdraw. In addition, the consent form included the contact (s) of the principal researcher for further inquiries.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Song, P., Liu, X., Cai, X. et al. Predictive analysis of college students’ academic procrastination behavior based on a decision tree model. Humanit Soc Sci Commun 11 , 869 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-024-03300-1

Download citation

Received : 13 June 2023

Accepted : 10 June 2024

Published : 03 July 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-024-03300-1

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

academic procrastination literature review

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • Children (Basel)
  • PMC10297372

Logo of children

Academic Procrastination in Children and Adolescents: A Scoping Review

Marcela paz gonzález-brignardello.

1 Department of Personality Psychology, Psychological Assessment and Treatment, Faculty of Psychology, Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia, UNED, 28040 Madrid, Spain; se.denu.isp@zelaznogpm (M.P.G.-B.); se.denu.isp@arivle-zehcnasa (A.S.-E.P.)

Angeles Sánchez-Elvira Paniagua

M. Ángeles lópez-gonzález.

2 Psychology Department, Faculty of Health Sciences, Universidad Rey Juan Carlos, 28922 Madrid, Spain

Associated Data

Data can be accessed via contacting the authors upon approval.

Academic procrastination is a persistent behavior in students’ academic development consisting of postponing or delaying the completion of necessary tasks and having a deadline for completion, which is associated with detriment in performance, school dropout, and loss of student well-being. The largest body of existing knowledge on this behavior comes from studies conducted with university students, although it is necessary to deepen the findings obtained at lower educational levels. The aim of this work has been to carry out a scoping review of the empirical publications focused on academic procrastination in children and adolescents. The inclusion and exclusion criteria are detailed following the general guidelines of the Joanna Briggs Institute. However, some modifications are incorporated in the flowchart to guide the review sequence. The search was conducted in eleven thematic (ERIC, MedLine, Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection, PsycINFO, PubPsych, and Teacher Reference Center) and multidisciplinary databases (Academic Search Ultimate, E-Journals, ProQuest, Scopus, and Web of Science) to identify relevant publications up to 2022, including grey literature. Out of the initial 1185 records screened, a total of 79 records were selected. The search results included a total of 79 records. The most used assessment instruments, the most studied variables, and the type of design and sources of information used in the selected studies are detailed. Cultural aspects that open new lines of future research are identified.

1. Introduction

Procrastination is a very common and pervasive behavior in different areas of human activity. It involves the intentional delay of actions and behaviors that have a time limit within which they should be completed [ 1 , 2 ]. There is a tendency to assume that everyone procrastinates or delays some necessary activity (e.g., medical appointments, exercise, paying fines, going to bed, among many other possible activities). However, to the extent that this behavior occurs on a frequent basis, it has consequences for those who engage in it, and may even affect others if it occurs, for example, in work or collaborative learning environments. Contrary to popular belief, people who engage in this behavior have a desire to complete tasks or actions, but have difficulty in translating these intentions into implementation actions, initiation, and completion [ 3 ].

Procrastination is a complex, poorly understood behavior that involves different cognitive, emotional, and behavioral components [ 4 , 5 , 6 ]. It has been understood as a failure of self-regulation [ 4 ], an avoidance behavior toward unpleasant tasks [ 7 ], due to fear of failure [ 8 ], fear of success [ 9 ], or an expression of poor action control [ 9 ], and it has been consistently associated with low self-efficacy, e.g., [ 10 ].

Academic procrastination, a type of domain-specific behavior, refers to the tendency of students to delay or postpone completing academic tasks, such as studying for an exam, doing homework, or writing an essay, even though they know they should perform these actions and have a specific deadline for completion. Academic procrastination has garnered significant attention from researchers, primarily due to two factors: (a) Its high prevalence among university students and, on the other hand, (b) the ease of access to samples of students. Approximately 80% of college students are estimated to procrastinate, making it one of the most prevalent issues among post-secondary students, with estimates ranging from 10% to 70% [ 11 , 12 ]. Contrary to the previous statement, little research has been conducted to understand the characteristics of procrastination in younger age groups, e.g., [ 13 ]. However, researchers became interested in studying the behavior of children and adolescents during the prolonged periods of confinement due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Research in relation to procrastination focused primarily on the significant increase in the use of electronic devices and social media, as well as the procrastination of academic activities that were mandatory online during that period, e.g., [ 14 , 15 , 16 ].

Academic procrastination leads to a decline in students’ well-being. It has been associated with poor academic performance [ 17 ], emotional distress (stress, anxiety, and depression) [ 18 , 19 , 20 ], and physical health deterioration, e.g., [ 21 , 22 ].

One of the many unanswered questions regarding academic procrastination pertains to its development in students. Is academic procrastination a behavioral pattern that develops at an early age, or does it emerge as a reaction or response as students face the transition to university level? If the behavior does develop at an early age, what is its prevalence in primary and secondary education, what role do parents and the education system play in the development of the problem, and are there interventions to change children’s procrastinating behavior? If so, what factors are involved in promoting change?

The aim of this work has been to carry out a panoramic review of the empirical publications focused on academic procrastination in children and adolescents. We propose a scoping review that will also allow us: (a) To identify the production and evolution of publications on academic procrastination in primary and secondary education; (b) to specify the methodological basic characteristics of the studies; (c) to conduct a content analysis to categorize the correlates investigated in relation to academic procrastination in this age group and to determine the types of interventions reported (see Figure 1 ). The ultimate goal, which is the essence of a systematic scoping review, is to detect gaps in research to contribute to future lines of inquiry.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is children-10-01016-g001.jpg

Flowchart of the specific objectives.

Following the methodology implemented in previous studies [ 23 , 24 ], this review includes several control mechanisms designed to reduce any bias that may exist a priori, as suggested by the PRISMA-ScR [ 25 ] (see Appendix A ) and the JBI Evidence Synthesis Manual [ 26 ]. The study was registered in the international registry for overview reviews and the protocol is available from OSF if required ( https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/KCXJ9 , accessed on 11 May 2023). Therefore, we developed a protocol that has allowed for uniform criteria to be applied to each of the registries, from the initial search for papers to the inclusion of the final papers.

2.1. Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

Based on the research question and following an adaptation of the PICO strategy, a protocol called “d-Cocospe” (documents, concept, context, studies, participants, and evaluation) was designed following the indications of various authors, e.g., [ 27 ] (see Table 1 ).

Inclusion and exclusion criteria.

CriteriaInclusionExclusion
Documents (d)Journal articles, books, book chapters, and doctoral thesesMagazine articles, editorials, conferences, etc.
Concept (Co)Academic procrastinationThe rest
Context (Co)Academic contextOther contexts (e.g., general, work or health procrastination)
Studies (s)Empirical studiesTheoretical reviews and case studies
Participants (p)Students under 18 yearsUniversity students or community samples
Evaluation (e)Behavioral or reported procrastination measures (i.e., self-reports or hetero reports)Single-item instruments or unstructured instruments

2.1.1. Documents

Both periodical (journal articles) and non-periodical (books, book chapters, and doctoral theses) publications were included. Magazine articles, editorials, conferences, and other similar types of documents were excluded.

2.1.2. Concept

The focus of the study was on publications in which academic procrastination was assessed.

2.1.3. Context

The study focused specifically on publications that assessed academic procrastination within an academic context. As a result, publications on general procrastination or on procrastination in other contexts (such as work, health, or leisure activities) were excluded.

2.1.4. Studies

Empirical studies were included and theoretical studies, literature reviews, and case studies were not considered.

2.1.5. Participants

The study subjects were exclusively students under 18 years and publications that included university students, community samples or those that did not specify the age or educational level of the participants were discarded.

2.1.6. Evaluation

Empirical studies in which the sample size and mode of assessment of procrastination was explicitly stated were included, if procrastination was assessed by behavioral or reported procrastination measures (i.e., self-reports or hetero reports). On the other hand, publications where procrastination was assessed by single-item instruments or unstructured instruments were excluded.

2.2. Search Strategy

The search equation was developed by the authors (MPGB, ASEP, and MALG) through initial exploratory searches in collaboration with expert documentalists from the Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (UNED).

The final search was constructed by applying Boolean operators (AND & OR) and truncation (* and inverted commas) in TI, AB, KW: (“academic procrast*” OR “student procrast*”) AND (child* OR adolesc* OR young OR teen* OR school OR infan* OR boy* OR girl* OR junior OR kid)] including publications up to December 2022. In the ProQuest database, NOFT was used: [(“academic procrast*” OR “student procrast*”) AND (child* OR adolesc* OR young OR teen* OR school OR infan* OR boy* OR girl* OR junior OR kid)]. For WoS, the search was executed in TOPIC: [(“academic procrast*” OR “student procrast*”) AND (child* OR adolesc* OR young OR teen* OR school OR infan* OR boy* OR girl* OR junior OR kid)] including publications up to December 2022.

2.3. Sources of Information

The documentary search was carried out in July 2022. Subsequently, in March 2023, the references were updated, and the records found up to December 2022 were included to replicate the search and incorporate new records.

2.3.1. Formal Strategies

The records were obtained through different sources of information. First, 11 automated databases were consulted: (a) Thematic databases in the areas of Psychology and Education: ERIC, MedLine, Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection, PsycINFO, PubPsych, and Teacher Reference Center, and (b) multidisciplinary: Academic Search Ultimate, E-Journals, ProQuest, Scopus, and Web of Science. The search was conducted without language restrictions.

Second, the search was complemented by consulting documents located in different international repositories, such as Redined, Cogprints, Zenodo, BASE, NDLTD Networked Digital Library of Theses and Dissertations, OAIster, and arxiv. Bibliographic references were also analyzed to identify possible publications.

2.3.2. Informal Strategies

Academic social networks (e.g., ResearchGate, academia.edu, Dimensions, etc.) were also explored to locate publications by relevant researchers in the field.

2.3.3. Retrospective Strategies

The search was complemented by analysis of systematic reviews and meta-analyses to retrieve potentially relevant articles.

The searches were conducted without applying language restrictions to control for possible linguistic bias. In this regard, online translation (DeepL and Google Translator) of the original documents was used when necessary (e.g., texts in Turkish, Farsi, Chinese, Indonesian, etc.).

2.4. Coding and Identification of Records and Data Extraction

All records obtained from each database were exported to separate libraries in the EndNote 20.5 software package. EndNote 20.5 allowed for the files to be merged into a single library, which facilitated the removal of duplicate items. The records were then exported to a shared spreadsheet in Google Drive, in order that the authors could work collaboratively. After several online meetings, a protocol was established to create the analysis fields for processing each record (data charting). Then, a series of bibliometric data were included: (a) Year of publication; (b) authorship; (c) title; (d) journal or book name; (e) DOI; and (f) abstract. In addition, several enriched fields were added in accordance with the d-Cocospe format: (a) Document typology: Journal articles, books, book chapters, theses, etc.; (b) Concept: It was specified whether the document dealt with procrastination. If this concept was not attended, the topic of analysis was specified; (c) Context: In each record it was indicated whether procrastination was confined to the academic field, otherwise, the specific context was indicated; (d) Study: The type of study was indicated, specifying whether it was an empirical study, theoretical study, review or case study; (e) Participants: The following information was collected: (i) Number of participants in the study; (ii) type of participants: Students, general population, young volunteers, etc.; (iii) education level: Primary education (children aged 6–7 to 11–12) or secondary education (students aged 12 to 17–18); (iv) educational grade or rank; and (v) geographical origin of the sample; (f) Evaluation: Information on the assessment instrument used to measure academic procrastination was included; (g) Type of design: Experimental, quasi-experimental, ex post facto or observational; (h) Type of intervention: Content of the interventions, type of format (individual vs. group), intervention setting, structure of each session, and record of measures taken; (i) Analyzed variables: The variables analyzed were categorized according to different domains: Sociodemographic domain, learning domain, health domain, relational domain, and intrapersonal domain; (j) Outcomes; and (k) Conclusions in relation to academic procrastination.

During the coding and data extraction process, regular meetings were held to discuss inconsistencies, doubts, and disagreements. Each reference was independently analyzed by two researchers (MPGB and MALG), and both relevant and non-relevant records were recorded, in order that all the items were coded after reading the title, abstract, and full text.

The next phase of the review consisted of leading two types of analysis: (a) Thematic content analysis of the variables, and (b) classification of experimental interventions based on the analysis of the variables under study.

To carry out the thematic content analysis, a sequential procedure was followed: (a) Generation of a list of variables measured in the documents’ titles, abstracts, and full texts; (b) configuration of initial categories (a posteriori) through inductive analysis; (c) creation of a category tree, grouping them into conceptual families; and (d) re-labeling and reducing categories to a more inclusive level.

The intervention classification was carried out by categorizing the fields where the intervention was directed. Then, for each study, the type of technique or program implemented was collected. The type of design, variables, and most relevant results obtained were recorded.

Finally, we present the results using summary tables and figures created with Microsoft Excel (version 16.7), MapChart, MIRO (online whiteboard), and Infogram, along with a narrative description of the main findings.

3.1. Production and Evolution of Publications

The search strategies allowed us to identify 1185 documents, of which 79 met the criteria for inclusion, representing 6.67% of the initial records. Figure 2 shows the entire process carried out from the selection of formal and informal strategies used, as well as the number of records and the reasons for inclusion and exclusion of each document in each phase of the screening process.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is children-10-01016-g002.jpg

Flowchart of the review process. The literature search has been conducted using formal strategies (automated databases and references from selected documents), informal strategies (academic social networks) and retrospective strategies [ 4 , 28 , 29 , 30 , 31 ].

On the other hand, Figure 3 shows the diachronic evolution of publications on academic procrastination in students under 18 years of age. The first selected work was published in 1995, although the evolution of publications has been irregular until the last 15 years, with the peak of records in 2021 with 15 documents.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is children-10-01016-g003.jpg

Diachronic evolution of the literature on academic procrastination in children and adolescents. In this graphic, it have been indicated the most cited refs. [ 32 , 33 , 34 ].

A comparison is made between the initially retrieved publications and the documents finally included. Similarly, the three publications with the highest number of citations in the Web of Science database in 2023 are labeled and the only experimental studies conducted to date are marked.

3.2. Characteristics of the Studies

Using the prototypical structure of the “Methods” section of any scientific publication, the characteristics of the studies are presented below: Participants, assessment, and type of design (see Appendix B ).

3.2.1. Participants

In terms of sample size, a total of 34,563 participants were counted, with an average per study equal to 437.5 students (min. = 5 and max. = 1509); out of these, 8.86% were primary school students (participants in seven studies) and the rest, 91.14%, were secondary school students. In total, five of the studies analyzed had all-girl samples, e.g., [ 35 , 36 , 37 , 38 , 39 ] and one of them was aimed at studying academic procrastination in girls with learning disabilities, e.g., [ 38 ] (see Figure 4 for details).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is children-10-01016-g004.jpg

Infographic: Summary of study methodology.

Regarding the geographical distribution of the samples, Figure 5 specifies the number of publications in each of the continents and countries. Moreover, it lists the countries in which experimental and quasi-experimental studies have been carried out.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is children-10-01016-g005.jpg

Geographical distribution of samples in studies on academic procrastination in children and adolescents. Note: It should be highlighted that the number of publications does not correspond to the final count due to the presence of a cross-cultural study which includes two different countries.

3.2.2. Instruments for Assessing Academic Procrastination

In Table 2 , the 26 different assessment tools used in the retrieved documents are shown. In general, these are specific questionnaires and scales directly applied to children and adolescents (self-reports), although the participation of parents and teachers (hetero reports) is also reported.

Recount of instruments used.

InstrumentCount
Procrastination Assessment Scale-Student—PASS (Solomon and Rothblum, 1984 [ ])23
Academic Procrastination Scale—AP-S (Çakıcı, 2003 [ ])10
Tuckman Procrastination Scale—TPS (Tuckman, 1991 [ ])8
Aitken Procrastination Inventory—API (Aiken, 1982 [ ])5
General Procrastination Scale—GPS (Lay, 1986 [ ])5
Escala de Procrastinación Académica—EPA (Busko, 1998 [ ])4
Academic Procrastination Questionnaire (Huang, 2009 [ ])2
Scale developed by authors (Dietz et al., 2007 [ ])3
Academic Procrastination Scale—APS-S (McCloskey, 2011 [ ])2
Academic Procrastination Inventory for Middle School Students—API-MSS (Zuo, 2020 [ ])1
Academic Procrastination Questionnaire (Ran, H., 2010 [ ])1
Academic Procrastination Scale—MSLQ (Lay and Silverman, 1996 [ ])1
Academic Procrastination Scale—APS (Lay, 1986 [ ])1
Academic Procrastination Student Form—APS (Milgram and Amir, 1998 [ ])1
Academic Procrastination Survey (Savari, K., 2011 [ ])1
Cuestionario de Procrastinación en el Estudio (CPE; Rosário et al., [ ])3
Irrational Procrastination Scale—IPS (Steel, 2010 [ ])1
Melbourne Decision Making Questionnaire (five items procrastination) (Mann et al., 1997 [ ])1
Scale developed by authors (Depreeuw and Lens, 1998 [ ])2
Scale developed by authors (Santyasa et al., 2020 [ ])1
Scale developed by authors (Shih, 2016 [ ])
Scale developed by authors (Ocak and Karatas, 2019 [ ])1
Academic Procrastination Scale—DPS (Ferrari et al., 1995 [ ])1
Total79

The authors indicate that the instruments used were adapted to the language of the sample; however, they are not questionnaires created specifically for children and adolescents, but mostly validated scales in university students that are also applied in individuals under 18 years old.

The most used instrument is the Procrastination Assessment Scale-Student/PASS, but typically only the first part of the scale, which is designed to measure the frequency of procrastination, is utilized.

3.2.3. Methodology of the Studies and Type of Design

In 97.47% of the studies, a quantitative methodology was employed, while 2.53% utilized qualitative or mixed method approaches. Regarding the design of quantitative studies, 86.07% were ex post facto designs (with an average sample size of 458.64 students), encompassing correlational or group comparison analyses. Additionally, 11.39% of the studies were experimental and quasi-experimental designs, including six randomized controlled trials (RCTs), two randomized non-controlled trials (RNCTs), and one pre-experimental study, with an average sample size of 197.5 students (see Figure 4 ). Within the ex post facto studies, correlation and regression analyses were predominantly observed, with a recent emergence of analyses based on structural equation modeling (SEM) in recent years.

Regarding studies that incorporate qualitative methodology, they have focused on children’s opinions about behaviors related to procrastination. In one of them [ 59 ], children were asked about the reasons for procrastination, strategies to avoid it, and suggestions to reduce this behavior. In this sense, boys mentioned playing computer games as a substitute behavior for studying, while girls preferred activities, such as reading books. The problematic use of mobile phones and the internet has also been studied (e.g., [ 59 ]). In this case, students considered access to information as a positive aspect of using the internet, and time loss as a negative aspect.

3.3. Content Analysis

3.3.1. investigated correlates.

The thematic content analysis reveals four main dimensions: Parental and teacher variables (teacher and family), personal variables (sociodemographic, personality, motivation, emotional/affective), mental and well-being (psychopathology, alternative or competitive behaviors, and well-being and quality of life), and variables of the learning cycle (self-regulated learning strategies, performance, and school).

In Figure 6 , the set of variables analyzed in the documents (i.e., those that have been measured) is detailed. The most studied variables correspond to dimensions related to the learning cycle, and self-regulated learning occupied the focus of most of this dimension. Thirty-four studies focused on variables related to this process, specifically self-regulated learning, e.g., [ 60 , 61 , 62 ], time management, e.g., [ 57 , 63 , 64 ], and inattention or management of distractions, e.g., [ 62 , 65 ]. Regarding previous learning experience and performance [ 33 ], we analyzed the role of experience in receiving low grades on exams and assignments and its relationship with academic procrastination. All studies that included performance and academic achievement variables, a total of 22, e.g., [ 52 , 66 , 67 ] were included in this category.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is children-10-01016-g006.jpg

Categories and analyzed variables.

On the other hand, studies that relate personality variables to academic procrastination are also among the most numerous, with a total of 31, highlighting self-efficacy or self-efficacy beliefs, e.g., [ 62 , 68 , 69 , 70 , 71 , 72 ], perfectionism, e.g., [ 57 , 68 , 73 , 74 , 75 ], and self-esteem, e.g., [ 33 , 67 ]. Other less common constructs are resilience, locus of control, and persistence.

The relationship between emotional and affective variables with academic procrastination was studied in 11 publications, with test anxiety being the most frequent variable, e.g., [ 75 ]; but not only related to evaluation, but also general academic anxiety, anxiety in the study process or for specific subjects, e.g., [ 32 , 76 ].

A specific area of study in this population of children and adolescents is parental support and the development of autonomy; a total of 18 publications analyzed variables related to the quality of parental relationships, parenting style, and parental involvement in education, e.g., [ 72 , 76 , 77 , 78 , 79 ], attention to the promotion of autonomy or excessive parental control, e.g., [ 78 , 79 ].

Alternative behaviors to studying, such as mobile phone use or internet browsing, have also been of interest to researchers, with nine publications dedicated to exploring the relationship between academic procrastination and these types of behaviors, e.g., [ 14 , 16 , 80 , 81 ].

Among the retrieved documents, there are also publications that study academic procrastination in the context of psychopathology, such as three studies that analyze procrastination behavior associated with addictive behaviors related to the internet, use of social networks, and electronic devices, e.g., [ 82 , 83 , 84 ], social anxiety disorder [ 80 , 82 ], and obsessive-compulsive symptoms [ 70 ].

3.3.2. Types of Interventions

Regarding the retrieved experimental studies and considering the intervention area, a total of nine studies were selected. These studies can be classified, according to their practical perspective, as follows: Healthcare (1); educational (2); psychoeducational (4); psychotherapeutic (1); and mixed (1).

From a broad socio-sanitary perspective, with an emphasis on child health within social, family, and educational context, Kocoglu and Emiroğlu [ 85 ] studied the impact of a school nursing service on the academic performance of 4th-grade students. The authors reported an increase in academic performance, while decreasing absenteeism and academic procrastination behaviors.

On the other hand, from an educational framework, Santyasa et al. [ 56 ] compared two teaching methods in two groups of students: A project-based learning (PjBL) model and a direct instruction (DI) model. They found that the project-based method improved academic performance, but this occurred primarily in the group of students with low levels of academic procrastination. From the same perspective, Dong and Izadpanah [ 35 ] compared the effect of corrective feedback after formative feedback in an experimental group with control group (women), obtaining improvement in academic resilience, educational belonging, and reduction in procrastination compared to the control group.

From a psycho-educational framework, Ghadampour [ 86 ] trained a group of female students in learning strategies (cognitive and metacognitive): The experimental group showed a reduction in procrastination and an increase in self-efficacy that was maintained during the follow-up phase, with significantly differential effects compared to the control group without intervention. Additionally, Motie [ 87 ] reported a study based on training in self-regulated learning strategies (boys) in an experimental group design with a control group, which resulted in a decrease in procrastination behavior in the experimental group. Moreover, from the psycho-educational framework, Jaradat [ 88 ] reported a study based on three groups: One assigned to cognitive therapy, study skills counseling, and a control group (waiting list). The two experimental groups improved satisfaction with studying in the post-test. The study skills counseling group showed a greater reduction in academic procrastination than the CT group, which decreased exam anxiety but not procrastination. Finally, Yildiz and Iskender [ 89 ] implemented a psycho-educational program aimed at secure attachment style in a control group (13 years old) that decreased intolerance of uncertainty and academic procrastination in the experimental group, compared to the control group without intervention, with effects that persisted after the program ended.

From a clinical psychological perspective, Lubis and Djuwita [ 37 ] intervened online and in a group format, through a CBT program aimed at academic procrastination, with five girls during the COVID-19 pandemic, with a pre-post and follow-up design. The authors reported that the intervention decreased levels of procrastination.

From a mixed perspective, Kheirkhah [ 36 ] trained girls in self-efficacy and learning strategies during eight sessions. The intervention significantly decreased procrastination compared to the control group without intervention.

4. Discussion

The aim of this work has been to carry out a scoping review of the empirical publications focused on academic procrastination in children and adolescents.

Regarding the first objective, which aims to identify the production and evolution of publications on academic procrastination in primary and secondary education, the results indicate a lack of significant interest from researchers in this topic.

In contrast to academic procrastination in university students [ 17 , 29 , 90 , 91 , 92 ], there has been a clear increase in the number of publications since 2020. In fact, 45.5% of the retrieved records are dated after this year. It is worth noting that 2020 was the year of the pandemic, with widespread confinement and the consequent implementation of online education systems at all levels [ 93 ], as well as a significant rise in the use of social media. Therefore, it is highly likely that this newfound interest has been influenced by the circumstances that occurred. Only time will tell if this trend continues in the coming years.

Regarding the second objective of identifying the common methodological characteristics of the studies, the most relevant data showed that academic procrastination has been predominantly studied in secondary education (92%). In terms of sample sizes, it is noteworthy that 90% of the studies were conducted with very large (>1000) or large (>350 students) samples. It is striking that only 11.4% of the studies were experimental in nature; the majority of the research has focused on exploring relationships between variables. Hopefully, in the future, there will be initiatives aimed at detecting and specifically intervening in this age group.

A high proportion of the retrieved studies came from Eastern countries, with a very limited presence of studies from Europe and America. These findings are similar to those reported by Lu et al. [ 40 ], in their meta-analysis on sociodemographic characteristics and procrastination, the authors found a large sample of studies with Chinese participants. On the other hand, Mann [ 94 ], in an exploratory study on cultural differences in decision-making in six cultures (USA, Australia, New Zealand, Japan, Hong Kong, and Taiwan), found that Asian students scored higher in academic procrastination compared to Western students, which could justify the differential interest shown by Eastern researchers compared to Western ones. This imbalance is even more pronounced if we consider that there is an Anglo-Saxon bias in the location of international literature in this review.

Within the methodological aspects, the analysis of the measurement instruments used to assess academic procrastination in students under 18 years revealed that many of the scales commonly used have not been specifically created for this age group [ 40 ]. This can represent a methodological limitation and a challenge when interpreting results. This highlights the need to reflect on more appropriate evaluation methods for children. For example, the development of observational scales could be a promising approach.

Recording the third objective, which aimed to conduct a content analysis to categorize the correlates investigated in relation to academic procrastination and determine the types of interventions reported, four topics were identified. The content analysis yielded four topics, including constructs related to self-regulated learning [ 95 , 96 ] and a range of personality, motivational, e.g., [ 97 ], and emotional variables [ 98 , 99 ], which were expected to be present. Additionally, a third topic emerged concerning the relationships established with significant individuals and the influence of specific variables, such as parenting style, excessive demands, and overprotection on the development of autonomy and self-control in children. Furthermore, an existing but less developed topic pertains to the relationship between procrastination and physical and mental health variables. While this is an emerging area, likely linked to post-pandemic studies, it requires greater attention. We cannot forget that this relationship has been well-studied in academic procrastination in adults [ 22 , 82 ].

The intervention studies reviewed, despite being scarce, show great methodological diversity in terms of sample size and characteristics (e.g., exclusively girls), making it impossible to draw conclusions on effectiveness. This, however, goes beyond the scope of this review. Nevertheless, given the insufficient number of studies found and the methodological problems detected, we can conclude that we are not yet close to conducting a meta-analysis focused exclusively on this age group and being able to answer specific questions.

Building on the previous discussion, we cannot only consider methodological issues related to assessing procrastination in children or the variables that explain the complexity of the construct, but also broader questions such as: What are the limits of the concept of procrastination in childhood? What is the alternative or competing behavior to completing necessary and obligatory tasks in children? Can we say that a child who is playing rather than doing their homework is procrastinating? On the other hand, should we ask ourselves if an inactive child who is not playing is also procrastinating? We believe that one of the important tasks of childhood is the development of play, pleasure, and a sense of fun, and we wonder when the “obligation of tasks” begins to give way to procrastination. The role of school in this regard seems to be very relevant, and it is logical to think that they have the possibility to detect and intervene early.

Is there procrastination in a childhood free of mandatory tasks? We can evoke hundreds of images of children demanding constant activity and insatiable curiosity. At what point, and due to what factors, does learning—or its tasks—become “procrastinatable”?

The analyzed studies do not address these issues. They also do not study, longitudinally, the transition through the stages or levels of study and the adaptation to the different changes and growing demands for autonomy, and how they impact procrastination behavior. In studies with university students, the relationship between academic procrastination and the change in the educational system has been discussed: The autonomy granted by higher education can be a stressor or a promoter of excessive flexibility, which in turn, may favor the emergence of procrastination behavior. In this regard, special attention has been paid to integration during the first year, due to the high dropout rates observed in this course and its relationship with academic self-efficacy, e.g., [ 97 ], use of self-regulation strategies, academic engagement, e.g., [ 100 ], and academic burnout, e.g., [ 101 ]. It is valid to question whether throughout primary and secondary education, the processes of change and transition between these systems present, in a similar way to what has been studied in university samples, critical moments related to the loss of self-regulatory processes and the consequent emergence of greater procrastination. Therefore, longitudinal and long-term studies are necessary.

This scoping review has inherent limitations associated with its scope and breadth, as it aims to provide a comprehensive overview of the literature rather than conduct a detailed assessment of individual study biases. Therefore, the assessment of individual study bias has not been conducted in this review. It is important to acknowledge these limitations when interpreting the findings and to consider them in future research.

5. Conclusions

In this study, we have found that there is a large proportion of publications reporting on the relationship between academic procrastination and personality, motivational, and self-regulation variables (both of learning and emotion) in childhood and adolescence. However, our analysis of the classification categories has allowed us to identify gaps in the literature, such as the need to define and operationalize the construct of procrastination in this age group and to develop appropriate assessment tools and techniques. Overall, it is not clear how prevalent the problem of academic procrastination is in this age group, while it is well-known to be a major problem in higher education, with broad consequences for psychological emotional and physical health, academic performance, and well-being.

Based on the studies analyzed here, the authors argue for the need to clarify, through different methodological strategies, the role of promoting autonomy, developing self-regulation and self-control in childhood, and how they relate to parenting styles, educational models, and teaching strategies. Emphasizing the possibility of early detection and intervention methods may pave the way for reducing the high prevalence of procrastination in university students.

The main conclusions are as follows:

  • Addressing academic procrastination in children and adolescents should consider both individual and contextual factors, as well as appropriate interventions;
  • There is a need for the development of more appropriate assessment tools to measure academic procrastination in children and adolescents, considering their specific developmental characteristics;
  • The prevalence of academic procrastination in this population is still understudied, highlighting a research gap that requires further attention;
  • In summary, further research and interventions are necessary to improve the understanding and management of academic procrastination in children and adolescents.

In order to enhance knowledge in this field, it is suggested to develop methodological improvements (such as the use of appropriate instruments, operationalization of the target behaviors under study, and high-quality research reporting) and promote the implementation of policies that provide specific financial support to foster collaboration between institutions and different knowledge areas.

Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR) Checklist [ 25 ].

SectionItemPRISMA-ScR Checklist ItemReported on Page # (Number)
Title
Title1Identify the report as a scoping review.1
Abstract
Structured summary2Provide a structured summary including, as applicable: Background, objectives,
eligibility criteria, sources of evidence, charting methods, results, and conclusions that relate to the review question(s) and objective(s).
1
Introduction
Rationale3Describe the rationale for the review in the context of what is already known. Explain why the review question(s)/objective(s) lend themselves to a scoping review approach.1–2
Objectives4Provide an explicit statement of the question(s) and objective(s) being addressed with reference to their key elements (e.g., population or participants, concepts, and context), or other relevant key elements used to conceptualize the review question(s) and/or objective(s)).2
Methods
Protocol, and registration5Indicate if a review protocol exists, if and where it can be accessed (e.g., web
address), and, if available, provide registration information including registration number.
3
Eligibility
criteria
6Specify the characteristics of the sources of evidence (e.g., years considered,
language, publication status) used as criteria for eligibility, and provide a rationale.
3, 6
Information sources7Describe all information sources (e.g., databases with dates of coverage, contact with authors to identify additional sources) in the search, as well as the date the most recent search was executed.4
Search8Present the full electronic search strategy for at least one database, including any limits used, in order that it could be repeated.4
Selection of sources of evidence9State the process for selecting sources of evidence (i.e., screening, eligibility) included.5
Data charting process10Describe the methods of charting data from the included sources of evidence (e.g., piloted forms, forms that have been tested by the team before their use, whether data charting was carried out independently, in duplicate) and any processes for obtaining and confirming data from investigators.5
Data items11List and define all variables for which data were sought and any assumptions and simplifications made.5
Critical appraisal of individual sources of
evidence
12If performed, provide a rationale for conducting a critical appraisal of included sources of evidence, describe the methods used, and how this information was used in any data
synthesis (if appropriate).
N/A
Summary
measures
13Not applicable for scoping reviews.
Synthesis of
results
14Describe the methods of handling and summarizing the data that were charted.5
Risk of bias
across studies
15Not applicable for scoping reviews.
Additional analyses16Not applicable for scoping reviews.
Results
Selection of sources of
evidence
17Give numbers of sources of evidence screened, assessed for eligibility, and included in the review, with reasons for exclusions at each stage, ideally using a flow diagram.6
Characteristics of
sources of evidence
18For each source of evidence, present characteristics for which data were charted and provide the citations.6, 7, 8, 10
Critical appraisal within sources of
evidence
19If performed, present data on critical appraisal of included sources of evidence (see item 12).
Results of individual
sources of evidence
20For each included source of evidence, present the relevant data that were charted that relate to the review question(s) and objective(s).
Synthesis of results21Summarize and/or present the charting results as they relate to the review question(s) and objective(s).5, 6, 7, 8, 9
Risk of bias across studies22Not applicable for scoping reviews.
Additional analyses23Not applicable for scoping reviews.
Discussion
Summary of evidence24Summarize the main results (including an overview of concepts, themes, and types of evidence available), explain how they relate to the review question(s) and objectives, and consider the relevance to key groups.11–12
Limitations25Discuss the limitations of the scoping review process.13
Conclusions26Provide a general interpretation of the results with respect to the review question(s) and objective(s), as well as potential implications and/or next steps.13
Funding
Funding27Describe sources of funding for the included sources of evidence, as well as sources of funding for the scoping review. Describe the role of the funders of the scoping review.14

Summary of Scoping Review.

Bibliometric InformationSampleDesign
AuthorYearDoc. TypeEdu LevelNCountry
Milgram et al. [ ]1995ArticleSS195ISREPF
Owens and Newbegin [ ]1997ArticlePS418AUSEPF
Milgram and Toubiana [ ]1999ArticleSS245ISREPF
Owens and Newbegin [ ]2000ArticleSS380AUSEPF
Davis [ ]2001ThesisSS284USAEPF
Nadeau et al. [ ]2003ArticleSS100CANEPF
Jaradat [ ]2004ThesisSS729JORRCT
Dietz and et al. [ ]2007ArticleSS704DEUEPF
Rosário et al. [ ]2008ArticleSS533/796PRTEPF
Klassen and Kuzucu [ ]2009ArticleSS508TUREPF
Klassen et al. [ ]2009ArticleSS612CAN, SGPEPF
Uzun Özer [ ]2009ArticleSS223TUREPF
Rosário et al. [ ]2009ArticleSS580/809PRTEPF
Al-Attiyah [ ]2010ArticlePS538QATEPF
Kalafat et al. [ ]2010ArticleSS285TUREPF
Kuhnle et al. [ ]2011ArticleSS348DEUEPF
Uzun Özer and Ferrari [ ]2011ArticleSS214TUREPF
Liu and Lu [ ]2011ArticleSS712CHNEPF
Hofer et al. [ ]2012ArticleSS697DEUEPF
Motie et al. [ ]2012ArticleSS250IRNEPF
Mih [ ]2013ArticleSS189ROUEPF
Motie et al. [ ]2013ArticleSS66IRNRCT
Bong et al. [ ]2014ArticleSS306KOREPF
Katz et al. [ ]2014ArticlePS171ISREPF
Tang et al. [ ]2014ArticleSS460CHNEPF
Hong et al. [ ]2015ArticleSS597TWNEPF
Jahromi et al. [ ]2015ArticleSS268IRNEPF
Gazidari et al. [ ]2016ArticleSS311IRNEPF
Ng [ ]2016ArticleSS442SGPEPF
Shih [ ]2016ArticleSS405TWNEPF
Zohreyi et al. [ ]2016ArticleSS210IRNEPF
Chen and Han [ ]2017ArticleSS577CHNEPF
Günlü and Ceyhan [ ]2017ArticleSS1088TUREPF
Kocoglu and Emiroglu [ ]2017ArticlePS31TURRNCT
Shih [ ]2017ArticleSS405TWNEPF
Yang et al. [ ]2017ArticleSS360CHNEPF
Borekci and Uyangor [ ]2018ArticleSS496TUREPF
Jiang et al. [ ]2018ArticleSS848KOREPF
Korkmaz et al. [ ]2018ArticleSS167TUREPF
Mostafa [ ]2018ArticleSS100EGYEPF
Ziegler and Opdenakker [ ]2018ArticleSS566NLDEPF
Ghadampour and Beiranvand [ ]2019ArticleSS30IRNRCT
Macias and Nevarez [ ]2019ArticleSS300MEXEPF
Ahmadi et al. [ ]2020ArticleSS149IRNEPF
Sula Atas and Kumcagiz [ ]2020ArticleSS602TUREPF
Durak [ ]2020ArticleSS451TUREPF
Eissa and Khalifa [ ]2020ArticleSS228EGYEPF
Gündüz [ ]2020ArticleSS358TUREPF
Lenggono and Tentama [ ]2020ArticleSS60IDNEPF
Saad [ ]2020ArticleSS68EGYEPF
Santyasa et al. [ ]2020ArticleSS124IDNRNCT
Tras and Gökçen [ ]2020ArticleSS599TUREPF
Trujillo-Chumán and Noé-Grijalva [ ]2020ArticleSS366PEREPF
Zhen et al. [ ]2020ArticleSS1505CHNEPF
Çam and Ögülmüs [ ]2021ArticleSS1068TUREPF
Fulano et al. [ ]2021ArticleSS1000MOZEPF
Hong et al. [ ]2021ArticleSS633CHNEPF
Khalifa [ ]2021ArticleSS250SAUEPF
Kheirkhah [ ]2021ArticleSS559IRNRCT
Niu et al. [ ]2021ArticleSS623CHNEPF
Opdenakker [ ]2021ArticleSS687NLDEPF
Quispe et al. [ ]2021ArticleSS36PEREPF
Rezaei and Zebardast [ ]2021ArticleSS350IRNEPF
Selçuk et al. [ ]2021ArticleSS1000TUREPF
Wang [ ]2021ArticleSS566CHNEPF
Xue et al. [ ]2021ArticlePS338CHNEPF
Yang et al. [ ]2021ArticleSS353CHNEPF
Yildiz and Iskender [ ]2021ArticleSS1068TURRCT
Yu et al. [ ]2021ArticleSS24CHNEPF
Asmali and Sayin [ ]2022ArticlePS245TUREPF
Chaji and Ebrahimpour [ ]2022ArticlePS662CHNEPF
Dong and Izadpanah [ ]2022ArticleSS5IRNRCT
Galindo-Contreras and Olivas-Ugarte [ ]2022ArticleSS256PEREPF
Gezgin [ ]2022ArticleSS300TUREPF
Lubis and Djuwita [ ]2022ArticleSS1255IDNPE
Muarifah et al. [ ]2022ArticleSS429IDNEPF
Salluca et al. [ ]2022ArticleSS370PEREPF
Üztemur and Dinç [ ]2022ArticleSS136TUREPF
Zhao et al. [ ]2022ArticleSS514CHNEPF

Note: Edu Level (Educational Level): PS (Primary School) and SS (Secondary School). Country: AUS (Australia), CAN (Canada), CHN (China), DEU (Germany), EGY (Egypt), IDN (Indonesia), IRN (Iran), ISR (Israel), JOR (Jordan), KOR (South Korea), MEX (Mexico), MOZ (Mozambique), NLD (Netherlands), PER (Peru), PRT (Portugal), QAT (Qatar), ROU (Romania), SAU (Saudi Arabia), SGP (Singapore), TUR (Turkey), TWN (Taiwan), and USA (United States). Design: EPF (Ex Post Facto), PE (Pre-Experimental study), RCT (Randomized Controlled Trial), and RNCT (Randomized non-Controlled Trial).

Funding Statement

This research received no external funding.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.P.G.-B., A.S.-E.P. and M.Á.L.-G.; methodology, M.P.G.-B. and M.Á.L.-G.; software, M.Á.L.-G.; formal analysis, M.P.G.-B. and M.Á.L.-G.; investigation, M.P.G.-B. and M.Á.L.-G.; resources, M.P.G.-B., A.S.-E.P. and M.Á.L.-G.; data curation, M.P.G.-B. and M.Á.L.-G.; writing—original draft preparation, M.P.G.-B. and M.Á.L.-G.; writing—review and editing, M.P.G.-B., A.S.-E.P. and M.Á.L.-G.; visualization, M.P.G.-B., A.S.-E.P. and M.Á.L.-G.; supervision, M.P.G.-B. and M.Á.L.-G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Data availability statement, conflicts of interest.

The authors have no conflict of interest to declare.

Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

  • Search Menu

Sign in through your institution

  • Advance articles
  • Themed Collections
  • Editor's Choice
  • Ilona Kickbusch Award
  • Supplements
  • Author Guidelines
  • Submission Online
  • Open Access Option
  • Self-Archiving Policy
  • About Health Promotion International
  • Editorial Board
  • Advertising and Corporate Services
  • Journals on Oxford Academic
  • Books on Oxford Academic

Health Promotion International

Ethics in Patients’ Health Literacy: a scoping review and a critical discussion

ORCID logo

  • Article contents
  • Figures & tables
  • Supplementary Data

Melina Evripidou, Areti Efthymiou, Venetia Velonaki, Athina Kalokairinou, Evridiki Papastavrou, Ethics in Patients’ Health Literacy: a scoping review and a critical discussion, Health Promotion International , Volume 39, Issue 4, August 2024, daae100, https://doi.org/10.1093/heapro/daae100

  • Permissions Icon Permissions

A growing body of literature has acknowledged that a high number of populations with low Health Literacy (HL) is related to poor health outcomes, inequities in healthcare and high economic costs. Those findings have formulated the research questions of this review: (i) what ethical issues arise within the context of patients’ HL and (ii) What is the relationship between HL and quality of life? This review followed the guidelines of Joanna Briggs Institute (JBI) and the Preferred Reporting Items for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR) and it was conducted in five databases: PubMed, CINAHL, MEDLINE, Scopus and Science Direct between June 2022 and December 2023. Out of the 3164 titles retrieved, 285 abstracts were eligible to proceed. Following a thorough examination of the full text of 61 papers, 45 sources were identified that met the inclusion criteria. The data analysis process was guided by the research questions, employing a thematic approach. Four themes were identified: the use of language and patient understanding, human rights, the principlism approach (justice, beneficence, non-maleficence and autonomy) and quality of life. The first theme mainly focused on the relation of HL with the notion of consent forms and national action plans. Human rights in relation to HL were discussed as a minor issue. The bioethical framework by Beauchamp and Childress (Principles of Biomedical Ethics, 6th edn. Oxford University Press, New York, NY, 2009), was addressed by several studies, with a particular focus on justice and the loss of autonomy. Quality of life indicated a positive correlation with HL by most of the authors, while few studies revealed a moderate correlation.

Personal account

  • Sign in with email/username & password
  • Get email alerts
  • Save searches
  • Purchase content
  • Activate your purchase/trial code
  • Add your ORCID iD

Institutional access

Sign in with a library card.

  • Sign in with username/password
  • Recommend to your librarian
  • Institutional account management
  • Get help with access

Access to content on Oxford Academic is often provided through institutional subscriptions and purchases. If you are a member of an institution with an active account, you may be able to access content in one of the following ways:

IP based access

Typically, access is provided across an institutional network to a range of IP addresses. This authentication occurs automatically, and it is not possible to sign out of an IP authenticated account.

Choose this option to get remote access when outside your institution. Shibboleth/Open Athens technology is used to provide single sign-on between your institution’s website and Oxford Academic.

  • Click Sign in through your institution.
  • Select your institution from the list provided, which will take you to your institution's website to sign in.
  • When on the institution site, please use the credentials provided by your institution. Do not use an Oxford Academic personal account.
  • Following successful sign in, you will be returned to Oxford Academic.

If your institution is not listed or you cannot sign in to your institution’s website, please contact your librarian or administrator.

Enter your library card number to sign in. If you cannot sign in, please contact your librarian.

Society Members

Society member access to a journal is achieved in one of the following ways:

Sign in through society site

Many societies offer single sign-on between the society website and Oxford Academic. If you see ‘Sign in through society site’ in the sign in pane within a journal:

  • Click Sign in through society site.
  • When on the society site, please use the credentials provided by that society. Do not use an Oxford Academic personal account.

If you do not have a society account or have forgotten your username or password, please contact your society.

Sign in using a personal account

Some societies use Oxford Academic personal accounts to provide access to their members. See below.

A personal account can be used to get email alerts, save searches, purchase content, and activate subscriptions.

Some societies use Oxford Academic personal accounts to provide access to their members.

Viewing your signed in accounts

Click the account icon in the top right to:

  • View your signed in personal account and access account management features.
  • View the institutional accounts that are providing access.

Signed in but can't access content

Oxford Academic is home to a wide variety of products. The institutional subscription may not cover the content that you are trying to access. If you believe you should have access to that content, please contact your librarian.

For librarians and administrators, your personal account also provides access to institutional account management. Here you will find options to view and activate subscriptions, manage institutional settings and access options, access usage statistics, and more.

Short-term Access

To purchase short-term access, please sign in to your personal account above.

Don't already have a personal account? Register

Email alerts

Citing articles via.

  • Recommend to Your Librarian
  • Journals Career Network

Affiliations

  • Online ISSN 1460-2245
  • Print ISSN 0957-4824
  • Copyright © 2024 Oxford University Press
  • About Oxford Academic
  • Publish journals with us
  • University press partners
  • What we publish
  • New features  
  • Open access
  • Rights and permissions
  • Accessibility
  • Advertising
  • Media enquiries
  • Oxford University Press
  • Oxford Languages
  • University of Oxford

Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University's objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide

  • Copyright © 2024 Oxford University Press
  • Cookie settings
  • Cookie policy
  • Privacy policy
  • Legal notice

This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an annual subscription.

Exploring 40 years on affective correlates to procrastination: a literature review of situational and dispositional types

  • Published: 14 January 2022
  • Volume 41 , pages 1097–1111, ( 2022 )

Cite this article

academic procrastination literature review

  • Reza Feyzi Behnagh   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-4109-3501 1 &
  • Joseph R. Ferrari 2  

3214 Accesses

10 Citations

1 Altmetric

Explore all metrics

The relationship between different emotions with situational (e.g., academic) and dispositional (chronic) procrastination was examined extensively in the literature since the early days of procrastination research. A review of empirical studies over the past 40 years might shed light on the role of emotions in procrastination in different contexts with different populations. The current paper reviewed 83 studies (from 1977 to 2021) exploring the relationship between 9 different emotions and situational and dispositional procrastination. The emotions examined, listed in the order of the extent of focus of scholarly research are: anxiety, fear, shame, guilt, regret, boredom, frustration, anger, and revenge. Findings highlight the important role of emotions as motives, antecedents, correlates, or consequences of situational and dispositional procrastination. Based on the findings, a lack of a comprehensive theory summarizing dispositional and situational procrastination is pointed out and avenues for future research are outlined and recommended.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

academic procrastination literature review

Does Rumination Mediate the Unique Effects of Shame and Guilt on Procrastination?

academic procrastination literature review

Understanding academic procrastination: A Longitudinal analysis of procrastination and emotions in undergraduate and graduate students

Procrastination and depression from a cognitive perspective: an exploration of the associations among procrastinatory automatic thoughts, rumination, and mindfulness, explore related subjects.

  • Artificial Intelligence

Data Availability

Data sharing not applicable to this article as no datasets were generated or analyzed during the current study.

Aitken, N. D. (1982). College student performance, satisfaction, and retention: Specification and estimation of a structural model. The Journal of Higher Education, 53 (1), 32–50. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221546.1982.11780423 .

Article   Google Scholar  

Ariani, D. W., & Susilo, Y. S. (2018). Why do it later? Goal orientation, self-efficacy, test anxiety, on procrastination. ECPS Journal, 17 , 45–73. https://doi.org/10.7358/ecps-2018-017-wahy .

Ashwin, G., Polizzi, C. P., & Mattson, R. E. (2019). Mindfulness, procrastination, and anxiety: Assessing their interrelationships. Psychology of consciousness: Theory, research, and practice . . https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000209 .

Book   Google Scholar  

Azure, J. A. (2011). Correlates of course anxiety and academic procrastination in higher education. Global Journal of Educational Research, 10 (1), 55–65.

Google Scholar  

Balkis, M. (2013). Academic procrastination, academic life satisfaction, and academic achievement: The mediating role of rational beliefs about studying. Journal of Cognitive and Behavioral Psychotherapies, 13 (1), 57–74.

Balkis, M., & Duru, E. (2019). Procrastination and rational/irrational beliefs: A moderated mediation model. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 37 (3), 299–315. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10942-019-00314-6 .

Berking, M., & Whitley, B. (2014). Affect regulation training (ART) Springer https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-1022-9 .

Berking, M., Wupperman, P., Reichardt, A., Pejic, T., Dippel, A., & Znoj, H. (2008). Emotion regulation skills as a treatment target in psychotherapy. Behavior Research and Therapy, 46 (11), 1230–1237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2008.08.005 .

Beswick, G., Rothblum, E. D., & Mann, L. (1988). Psychological antecedents of student procrastination. Australian Psychologist, 23 (2), 207–217. https://doi.org/10.1080/00050068808255605 .

Blunt, A., & Pychyl, T. A. (1998). Volitional action and inaction in the lives of undergraduate students: State orientation, procrastination, and proneness to boredom. Personality and Individual Differences, 24 (5), 837–846. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00018-X .

Blunt, A., & Pychyl, T. A. (2005). Task aversiveness and procrastination: A multi-dimensional approach to task aversiveness across stages of personal projects. Personality and Individual Differences, 28 , 153–167. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00091-4 .

Börsch-Supan, A. H., Bucher-Koenen, T., Hurd, M. D., & Rohwedder, S. (2018). Saving regret (No. w25238). National Bureau of Economic Research. https://doi.org/10.3386/w25238 .

Burka, J. B., & Yuen, L. M. (1983). Procrastination: Why you do it and what to do about it. Addison-Wesley.

Burka, J. B., & Yuen, L. M. (2008). Procrastination: Why you do it, what to do about it now. Da Capo Press.

Cohen, J. R., & Ferrari, J. R. (2010). Take some time to think this over: The relation between rumination, indecision, and creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 22 (1), 68–73. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400410903579601 .

Constantin, K., English, M. M., & Mazmanian, D. (2018). Anxiety, depression, and procrastination among students: Rumination plays a larger mediating role than worry. Journal of Rational Emotional Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy, 36 , 15–27. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10942-017-0271-5 .

Custer, N. (2018). Test anxiety and academic procrastination among prelicensure nursing students. Nursing Education Perspectives, 39 (3), 162–163.

Day, V., Mesnick, D., & O’Sullivan, M. (2000). Patterns of academic procrastination. Journal of College Reading and Learning, 30 , 120–134. https://doi.org/10.1080/10790195.2000.10850090 .

Diaz-Morales, J. F., Ferrari, J. R., Argumedo, D., & Diaz, K. (2006). Procrastination and demographic characteristics in Spanish adults: Further evidence. The Journal of Social Psychology, 146 (5), 629–633. https://doi.org/10.3200/SOCP.146.5.629-633 .

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Dunn, K. (2014). Why wait? The influence of academic self-regulation, intrinsic motivation, and statistics anxiety on procrastination in online statistics. Innovation Higher Education, 39 , 33–44. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10755-013-9256-1 .

Eckert, M., Ebert, D. D., Lehr, D., Sieland, B., & Berking, M. (2016). Overcoming procrastination: Enhancing emotion regulation skills reduce procrastination. Learning and Individual Differences, 52 , 10–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.10.001 .

Ellis, A., & Knaus, W. J. (1977). Overcoming procrastination . Institute for Rational Living.

Endler, N. S., Edwards, J. M., Vitelli, R., & Parker, J. D. (1989). Assessment of state and trait anxiety: Endler multidimensional Anxiety Scales. Anxiety Research, 2 (1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1080/08917778908249322 .

Farran, B. (2004). Predictors of academic procrastination in college students. (Publication No. 3125010) [Doctoral Dissertation, Fordham University]. ProQuest Dissertations Publishing.

Fee, R. L., & Tangney, J. P. (2000). Procrastination: A means of avoiding shame or guilt? Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 15 (5), 167–184.

Ferrari, J. R. (1989). Reliability of academic and dispositional measures of procrastination. Psychological Reports, 64 , 1057–1058. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1989.64.3c.1057 .

Ferrari, J. R. (1992). Psychometric validation of two procrastination inventories for adults: Arousal and avoidance measures. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 14 (2), 97–110. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00965170 .

Ferrari, J. R. (2000). Procrastination and attention: Factor analysis of attention deficit, boredomness, intelligence, self-esteem, and task delay frequencies. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 15 (5), 185–196.

Ferrari, J. R. (2010). Still procrastinating? The no-regrets guide to getting it done. Wiley.

Ferrari, J. R. (2011). Chronic procrastination: Life beyond ineffective time management. In A. P. Varga (Ed.), Time management (pp. 83–91). Nova Science Publishers. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-230-34371-9_22 .

Ferrari, J. R., & Beck, B. L. (1998). Affective responses before and after fraudulent excuses by academic procrastination. Education, 118 (4), 529–537.

Ferrari, J. R., & Emmons, R. A. (1994). Procrastination as revenge: Do people report using delays as a strategy for vengeance? Personality and Individual Differences, 17 (4), 539–544. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(94)90090-6 .

Ferrari, J. R., & Olivette, M. J. (1994). Parental authority and the development of female dysfunctional procrastination. Journal of Research in Personality, 28 , 87–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/jrpe.1994.1008 .

Ferrari, J. R., & Scher, S. J. (2000). Toward an understanding of academic and nonacademic tasks procrastinated by students: The use of daily logs. Psychology in the Schools, 34 , 359–366. https://doi.org/10.1002/1520-6807(200007)37:4%3c367::AID-PITS7%3e3.0.CO;2-Y .

Ferrari, J. R., & Tibbett, T. (2017). Procrastination. In V. Zeigler-Hill & T. K. Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences (pp. 1–8). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_2272-1 .

Ferrari, J. R., Parker, J. T., & Ware, C. B. (1992). Academic procrastination: Personality correlates with Myers-Briggs types, self-efficacy, and academic locus of control. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 7 (3), 495–502.

Ferrari, J. R., Johnson, J. L., & McCown, W. G. (1995). Procrastination and task avoidance: Theory, research, and treatment. Plenum Press . https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-0227-6 .

Ferrari, J. R., Keane, S. M., Wolfe, R. N., & Beck, B. L. (1998). The antecedents and consequences of academic excuse making: Examining individual differences in procrastination. Research in Higher Education, 39 (2), 199–215. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018768715586 .

Ferrari, J. R., O’Callaghan, J., & Newbegin, I. (2005). Prevalence of procrastination in the United States, United Kingdom, and Australia: Arousal and avoidance delays among adults. North American Journal of Psychology, 7 (1), 1–6.

Ferrari, J. R., Diaz-Morales, J. F., O’Callaghan, J., Diaz, K., & Argumedo, D. (2007). Frequent behavioral delay tendencies by adults: International prevalence rates of chronic procrastination. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38 (4), 458–464. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022107302314 .

Ferrari, J. R., Özer, B. U., & Demir, A. (2009). Chronic procrastination among Turkish adults: Exploring decisional, avoidant, and arousal styles. The Journal of Social Psychology, 149 (3), 302–307. https://doi.org/10.3200/SOCP.149.3.402-408 .

Ferrari, J. R., Barnes, K. L., & Steel, P. (2019). Life regrets by avoidant and arousal procrastinators. Journal of Individual Differences, 30 (3), 163–168. https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001.30.3.163 .

Flett, G. L., Blankstein, K. R., Hewitt, P. L., & Koledin, S. (1992). Components of perfectionism and procrastination in college students. Social Behavior and Personality, 20 (2), 85–94.

Flett, G. L., Blankstein, K. R., & Martin, T. R. (1995). Procrastination, negative self-evaluation, and stress in depression and anxiety. In J. Ferrari, J. L. Johnson, & W. McCown (Eds.), Procrastination and task avoidance: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 137–167). Plenum Publications. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-0227-6_7 .

Flett, A. L., Haghbin, M., & Pychyl, T. A. (2016). Procrastination and depression from a cognitive perspective: An exploration of the associations among procrastinatory automatic thoughts, rumination, and mindfulness. Journal of Rational Emotional Cognitive Behavior Therapy, 34 , 169–286. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10942-016-0235-1 .

Gadosey, C. K., Schnettler, T., Scheunemann, A., Fries, S., & Grunschel, C. (2021). The intraindividual co-occurrence of anxiety and hope in procrastination episodes during exam preparations: An experience sampling study. Learning & Individual Differences, 88 , 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2021.102013 .

Glick, D. M., Millstein, D. J., & Orsillo, S. M. (2014). A preliminary investigation of the role of psychological inflexibility in academic procrastination. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 3 , 81–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2014.04.002 .

Goroshit, M., Hen, M., & Ferrari, J. R. (2020). Life-domain regret regarding procrastination (LDR-P): Scale validation in the United States and Israel. Current Psychology, 39 (3), 900–912. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-9801-2 .

Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2 (3), 271–299. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271 .

Grunschel, C., Patrzek, J., & Fries, S. (2013). Exploring reasons and consequences of academic procrastination: An interview study. European Journal of Psychology Education, 28 , 841–861. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10212-012-0143-4 .

Haghbin, M., McCaffrey, A., & Pychyl, T. A. (2012). The complexity of the relation between fear of failure and procrastination. Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 30 , 249–263. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10942-012-0153-9 .

Harriett, J., & Ferrari, J. (1996). Prevalence of procrastination among samples of adults. Psychological Reports, 78 , 611–616. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1996.78.2.611 .

Harrington, N. (2005). It’s too difficult! Frustration intolerance beliefs and procrastination. Personality and Individual Differences, 39 , 873–883. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.12.018 .

Haycock, L. A., McCarthy, P., & Skay, C. L. (1998). Procrastination in college students: The role of self-efficacy and anxiety. Journal of Counseling & Development, 76 , 317–324. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6676.1998.tb02548.x .

Hen, M., & Goroshit, M. (2018). General and life-domain procrastination in highly educated adults in Israel. Frontiers in Psychology, 9 , 1–8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01173 .

Kim, K. R., & Seo, E. H. (2015). The relationship between procrastination and academic performance: A meta-analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 82 , 26–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.02.038 .

Klingsieck, K. B. (2013). Procrastination in different life domains: Is procrastination domain specific? Current Psychology, 32 , 175–185. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-013-9171-8 .

Knaus, W. J. (1979). Do it now: How to stop procrastinating . Prentice-Hall.

Kroese, F. M., de Ridder, D. T. D., Evers, C., & Adriaanse, M. A. (2014). Bedtime procrastination: Introducing a new area of procrastination. Frontiers in Psychology, 5 , 611–617. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00611 .

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Kroese, F. M., Nauts, S., Kamphorst, B. A., Anderson, J. H., & de Ridder D. T. D. (2016). Bedtime procrastination: A behavioral perspective on sleep insufficiency. In F. M. Sirois & T. A. Pychyl, (Eds.), Procrastination, health, and well-being (pp. 93–119). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-802862-9.00005-0 .

Kuhl, J. (1994). A theory of action and state orientations. In J. Kuhl & J. Bechmann (Eds.), Volition and personality: Action vs state orientation (pp. 9–46). Hegrefe and Huber.

Kuhnle, C., Hofer, M., & Kilian, B. (2011). The relationship of self-control, procrastination, motivational interference, and regret with school grades and life balance. Diskurs Kindheits-Und Jungendforschung, 1 , 31–44.

Lavoie, J. A. A., & Pychyl, T. A. (2001). Cyberslacking and the procrastination superhighway: A web-based survey of online procrastination, attitudes, and emotion. Social Science Computer Review, 19 (4), 431–444. https://doi.org/10.1177/089443930101900403 .

Lay, C. H. (1986). At last, my research on procrastination. Journal of Research in Personality, 20 , 474–495. https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(86)90127-3 .

Lay, C. H., & Silverman, S. (1996). Trait procrastination, anxiety, and dilatory behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 21 (1), 61–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(96)00038-4 .

Lay, C. H., Edwards, J. M., Parker, J. D. A., & Endler, N. S. (1989). An assessment of appraisal, anxiety, coping, and procrastination during an examination period. European Journal of Personality, 3 , 195–208. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2410030305 .

Lewis, H. B. (1971). Shame and guilt in neurosis. Psychoanalytic Review, 58 (3), 419–438.

PubMed   Google Scholar  

Yang, Z., Asbury, K., & Griffiths, M. D. (2019). An exploration of problematic smartphone use among Chinese university students: Associations with academic anxiety, academic procrastination, self-regulation, and subjective wellbeing. International Journal of Mental Health Addiction, 17 , 596–614. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-018-9961-1 .

Mann, L. (1982). Decision-making questionnaire . Flinders University of South Australia (Unpublished manuscript).

Mann, L. (2016). Procrastination revisited: A commentary. Australian Psychologist, 51 , 47–51. https://doi.org/10.1111/ap.12208 .

Mariani, M. G., & Ferrari, J. R. (2012). Adult inventory of procrastination Scale (AIP): A comparison of models with an Italian sample. TPM, 19 (1), 3–14.

Martinčeková, L., & Enright, R. D. (2020). The effects of self-forgiveness and shame-proneness on procrastination: Exploring the mediating role of affect. Current Psychology, 39 , 428–437. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-9926-3 .

McCown, W., & Johnson, J. (1989). Procrastination, a principal components analysis. Personality & Individual Differences, 10 , 197–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(89)90204-3 .

Milgram, N. A., & Tourbiana, Y. (1999). Academic anxiety, academic procrastination, and parental involvement in students and their parents. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 69 , 345–361. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709999157761 .

Milgram, N. A., Dangour, W., & Raviv, A. (1992). Situational and personal determinants of academic procrastination. The Journal of General Psychology, 119 (2), 123–133. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.1992.9921166 .

Milgram, N. A., Marshevsky, S., & Sadeh, C. (1995). Correlates of academic procrastination: Discomfort, task aversiveness, and task capability. The Journal of Psychology, 129 (2), 145–155. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1995.9914954 .

Muller, S. M., Wegmann, E., Stolze, D., & Brand, M. (2020). Maximizing social outcomes? Social zapping and fear of missing out mediate the effects of maximization and procrastination on problematic social network use. Computers in Human Behavior, 107 , 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106296 .

Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2004). Academic procrastination and statistics anxiety. Assessment and Evaluation in Higher Education, 29 (1), 3–19. https://doi.org/10.1080/0260293042000160384 .

Özer, B. U., Demir, A., & Ferrari, J. R. (2009). Exploring academic procrastination among Turkish students: Possible gender differences in prevalence and reasons. The Journal of Social Psychology, 149 (2), 241–257. https://doi.org/10.3200/SOCP.149.2.241-257 .

Pacht, A. (1984). Reflections on perfection. American Psychologist, 39 (4), 386–390. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.39.4.386 .

Panek, E. (2014). Left to their own devices: College students’ “guilty pleasure” media use and time management. Communication Research, 41 (4), 561–577. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650213499657 .

Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Titz, W., & Perry, R. P. (2002). Academic emotions in students' self-regulated learning and achievement: A program of quantitative and qualitative research. Educational Psychologist, 37 , 91–106.

Pekrun, R., Frenzel, A. C., Goetz, T., & Perry, R. (2007). The control-value theory of achievement emotions: An integrative approach to emotions in education. In P. A. Schutz & R. Pekrun (Eds.), Emotion in education (pp. 13–36). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012372545-5/50003-4 .

Przepiorka, A., Blanchnio, A., & Cudo, A. (2021). Procrastination and problematic new media use: The mediating role of future anxiety. Current Psychology . https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01773-wx .

Przybylski, A. K., Murayama, K., DeHaan, C. R., & Gladwell, V. (2013). Motivational, emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing out. Computers in Human Behavior, 29 , 1841–1848. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.014 .

Pychyl, T. A. (2013). Solving the procrastination puzzle: A concise guide to strategies for change. Penguin.

Pychyl, T. A., & Sirois, F. M. (2016). Procrastination, emotion regulation, and well-being. In F. M. Sirois & T. A. Pychyl (Eds.), Procrastination, health, and well-being (pp. 163–188). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-802862-9.00008-6 .

Pychyl, T. A., Lee, J., Thibodeau, R., & Blunt, A. (2000). Five days of emotion: An experience sampling study of undergraduate student procrastination. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 15 , 239–254.

Pyman, D. (2020). Academic procrastination, self-regulation, anxiety, and personality (ASAP): ‘Some’ day is not a day of the week [Master’s Thesis, Trent University]. Trent University Library & Archives. http://digitalcollections.trentu.ca/objects/etd-792 .

Rahardjo, W., & Juneman, S. Y. (2013). Computer anxiety, academic stress, and academic procrastination on college students. Journal of Education and Learning, 7 (3), 147–152. https://doi.org/10.11591/edulearn.v7i3.179 .

Rahimi, S. (2019). Understanding academic procrastination: A longitudinal analysis of procrastination and emotions in undergraduate and graduate students. (Publication no. 28254487) [Doctoral Dissertation, McGill University]. ProQuest Dissertations Publishing.

Reinecke, L., Hartmann, T., & Eden, A. (2014). The guilty couch potato: The role of ego depletion in reducing recovery through media use. Journal of Communication, 64 , 569–589. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12107 .

Rothblum, E. D., Solomon, L. J., & Murakami, J. (1986). Affective, cognitive, and behavioral differences between high and low procrastinators. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 33 (4), 387–394. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.33.4.387 .

Saklofske, D. H. (2018). Editorial. Personality and Individual Differences, 122 , A1–A2.

Saddler, C. D., & Buley, J. (1999). Predictors of academic procrastination in college students. Psychological Reports, 84 , 686–688. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1999.84.2.686 .

Sarason, I. G. (1972). Test anxiety and cognitive modeling. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28 (1), 58–61. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0035476 .

Scher, S. J., & Ferrari, J. R. (2000). The recall of completed and non-completed tasks through daily logs to measure procrastination. Journal of Social Behavior & Personality, 15 , 255–265.

Scher, S. J., & Osterman, N. M. (2001). Procrastination, conscientiousness, anxiety, and goals: Exploring the measurement and correlates of procrastination among school-aged children. Psychology in the Schools, 39 (4), 385–398. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.10045 .

Schouwenburg, H. C. (1992). Procrastinators and fear of failure: An exploration of reasons for procrastination. European Journal of Personality, 6 , 225–236. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2410060305 .

Schouwenburg, H. C. (1995). Academic procrastination: Theoretical notions, measurement, and research. In J. R. Ferrari, J. L. Johnson, & W. G. McCown (1995). Procrastination and task avoidance: Theory, research, and treatment, (pp. 71–95). Plenum Press. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-0227-6_4 .

Senecal, C., Koestner, R., & Vallerand, R. J. (1995). Self-regulation and academic procrastination. The Journal of Social Psychology, 135 (5), 607–619. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1995.9712234 .

Sirois, F. M., & Giguère, B. (2018). Giving in when feeling less good: Procrastination, action control, and social temptations. British Journal of Social Psychology, 57 , 404–427. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12243 .

Sirois, F. M., & Kitner, R. (2015). Less adaptive or more maladaptive? A meta-analytic investigation of procrastination and coping. European Journal of Personality, 29 (4), 433–444. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1985 .

Sirois, F. M., & Pychyl, T. A. (2013). Procrastination and the priority of short-term mood regulation: Consequences for future self. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7 (2), 115–127. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12011 .

Sirois, F. M., Nauts, S., & Molnar, D. S. (2019). Self-compassion and bedtime procrastination: An emotion regulation perspective. Mindfulness, 10 , 434–445. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-018-0983-3 .

Sirois, F. M., & Pychyl, T. A. (2016). Procrastination, health, and well-being Academic Press https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-18582-8 .

Solomon, L., & Rothblum, E. (1984). Academic procrastination: Frequency and cognitive-behavioral correlates. Journal of Counselling Psychology, 31 (4), 503–509. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.31.4.503 .

Spada, M. M., Hiou, K., & Nikcevic, A. V. (2006). Metacognitions, emotions, and procrastination. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly, 20 (3), 319–326. https://doi.org/10.1891/jcop.20.3.319 .

Stöber, J., & Joorman, J. (2001). Worry, procrastination, and perfectionism: Differentiating amount of worry, pathological worry, anxiety, and depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25 (1), 49–60. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026474715384 .

Subotnik, R., Steiner, C., & Chakraborty, B. (1999). Procrastination revisited: The constructive use of delayed response. Creativity Research Journal, 12 (2), 151–160. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1202_7 .

Sudler, E. L. (2014). Academic procrastination as mediated by executive functioning, perfectionism, and frustration intolerance in college students. (Publication No. 3575249) [Doctoral Dissertation. St. John's University]. ProQuest Dissertations Publishing.

Tangney, J. P. (1993). Shame and guilt. In C. G. Costello (Ed.), Symptoms of depression (pp. 161–180). Wiley.

Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P., Fletcher, C., & Gramzow, R. (1992). Shamed into anger? The relation of shame and guilt to anger and self-reported aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62 (4), 669–675. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.62.4.669 .

Tibbett, T. P., & Ferrari, J. R. (2018). The U.S. as a procrastiNATION: Assessing indecision on life satisfaction and life regret. North American Journal of Psychology, 20 (1), 111–116.

Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1997). Longitudinal study of procrastination, performance, stress, and health: The costs and benefits of dawdling. Psychological Science, 8 (6), 454–458. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00460.x .

Tice, D. M., & Bratslavsky, E. (2000). Giving in to feel good: The place of emotion regulation in the context of general self-control. Psychological Inquiry, 11 (3), 149–159. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1103_03 .

Tuckman, B. (1991). The development and concurrent validity of the procrastination scale. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 51 , 473–480. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164491512022 .

vanEerde, W. (2013). A meta-analytically derived nomological network of procrastination. Personality and Individual Differences, 35 , 1401–1418. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00358-6 .

Vodanovich, S. J., & Rupp, D. E. (1999). Are procrastinators prone to boredom? Social Behavior and Personality, 27 (1), 11–16. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.1999.27.1.11 .

Walsh, J., & Ugumba-Agwunobi, G. (2002). Individual differences in statistics anxiety: The roles of perfectionism, procrastination, and trait anxiety. Personality and Individual Differences, 33 , 239–251. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00148-9 .

Wan, H. C., Downey, L. A., & Stough, C. (2014). Understanding non-work presenteeism: Relationships between emotional intelligence, boredom, procrastination, and job stress. Personality and Individual Differences, 65 , 86–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.018 .

Wang, J., Wang, P., Yang, X., Zhang, G., Wang, X., Zhao, F., & Lei, L. (2019). Fear of missing out and procrastination as mediators between sensation seeking and adolescent smartphone addiction. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 17 (4), 1049–1062. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11469-019-00106-0 .

Wartberg, L., Thomasius, R., & Paschke, K. (2021). The relevance of emotion regulation, procrastination, and perceived stress for problematic social media use in a representative sample of children and adolescents. Computers in Human Behavior, 121 , 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2021.106788 .

Yerdelen, S., McCaffrey, A., & Klasse, R. M. (2016). Longitudinal examination of procrastination and anxiety, and their relation to self-efficacy for self-regulated learning: Latent growth curve modeling. Educational Sciences: Theory & Practice, 16 (1), 5–22. https://doi.org/10.12738/estp.2016.1.0108 .

Xu, P., Gonzalez-Vallejo, C., & Xiong, Z. H. (2016). State anxiety reduces procrastinating behavior. Motivation and Emotion, 40 , 625–637. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9554-x .

Download references

This material is based upon work supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant No. (1917949) awarded to the first author.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Educational Theory & Practice University at Albany, State University of New York, Albany, NY, USA

Reza Feyzi Behnagh

Department of Psychology, DePaul University, 2219 North Kenmore Avenue, Chicago, IL, 60614, USA

Joseph R. Ferrari

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Reza Feyzi Behnagh or Joseph R. Ferrari .

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and informed consent.

This is a literature review summarizing existing studies in the scholarly literature. This literature review does not entail any data collection from any human participant, and because of this no IRB approval was sought nor any informed consent obtained.

Conflict of Interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Both authors contributed to this article equally, and the author sequence was by coin flip.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Feyzi Behnagh, R., Ferrari, J.R. Exploring 40 years on affective correlates to procrastination: a literature review of situational and dispositional types. Curr Psychol 41 , 1097–1111 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02653-z

Download citation

Accepted : 20 December 2021

Published : 14 January 2022

Issue Date : February 2022

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02653-z

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Dispositional procrastination
  • Situational procrastination
  • Literature review
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

IMAGES

  1. Literature Review On Procrastination Impact On Academic Performance

    academic procrastination literature review

  2. Academic Procrastination Essay Example

    academic procrastination literature review

  3. (PDF) ACADEMIC PROCRASTINATION IN STUDENTS AND HOW TO OVERCOME IT

    academic procrastination literature review

  4. 2 Academic Procrastination

    academic procrastination literature review

  5. (PDF) Academic procrastination, family social support and academic

    academic procrastination literature review

  6. (PDF) Academic Procrastination

    academic procrastination literature review

COMMENTS

  1. Academic interventions for academic procrastination: A review of the

    The present review examined the recent literature on causes and consequences of academic procrastination and the limited number of studies of academic interventions for academic procrastination ...

  2. A Systematic Review of Interventions to Reduce Academic Procrastination

    Academic procrastination is a prevalent and pernicious self-regulation failure, which affects students' academic performance, health, and well-being. We conducted a systematic review of the recent (i.e., 2018 and subsequent) literature on the efficacy of interventions designed to reduce academic procrastination in several relevant online databases. Twenty-one studies, which matched our ...

  3. Understanding The Factors Influencing Academic Procrastination: A

    Academic procrastination is a common and detrimental behavior among students that negatively impacts academic performance and well-being. ... The literature review highlights the nega ve ...

  4. Interventions to reduce academic procrastination: A systematic review

    This research aims to systematize the scientific production on educational interventions aimed at reducing academic procrastination in the classroom. To this end, a systematic review of the literature (PRISMA-P) of the interventions carried out in the last decade ( n = 32) was conducted.

  5. Academic interventions for academic procrastination: A review of the

    Abstract. Procrastination is a widespread phenomenon in academic settings. It has been studied from many different theoretical angles, and a variety of causes and consequences have been suggested. Recent studies support the notion that academic procrastination can be seen from a situational perspective and as a failure in learning self-regulation.

  6. Academic interventions for academic procrastination: A review of the

    Recent studies support the notion that academic procrastination can be seen from a situational perspective and as a failure in learning self-regulation. ... S., & Hen, M. (2018). Academic interventions for academic procrastination: A review of the literature. Journal of Prevention & Intervention in the Community, 46(2), 117-130. https ...

  7. Academic interventions for academic procrastination: A review of the

    The present review examined the recent literature on causes and consequences of academic procrastination and the limited number of studies of academic interventions for academic procrastination. Findings of this review strengthen the need to further study the topic of academic interventions for academic procrastination and to develop effective ...

  8. Academic interventions for academic procrastination: A review of the

    The present review examined the recent literature on causes and consequences of academic procrastination and the limited number of studies of academic interventions for academic Procrastination to suggest that interventions should address situational as well as deficits in self-regulation to help students overcome their procrastinating tendencies. ABSTRACT Procrastination is a widespread ...

  9. Academic Procrastination in Children and Adolescents: A Scoping Review

    Academic procrastination is a persistent behavior in students' academic development consisting of postponing or delaying the completion of necessary tasks and having a deadline for completion, which is associated with detriment in performance, school dropout, and loss of student well-being. The largest body of existing knowledge on this behavior comes from studies conducted with university ...

  10. Interventions to Reduce Academic Procrastination: A Review of Their

    This chapter presents the results of the revision of scientific literature about psycho-educational and clinical approaches to reduce academic procrastination. ... Zacks, S., & Hen, M. (2018). Academic interventions for academic procrastination: A review of the literature. Journal of Prevention & Intervention in the Community, 46(2), 117-130 ...

  11. Study Habits and Procrastination: The Role of Academic Self-Efficacy

    The conceptual model, shown in. Figure 1. , assumes that the influence of Study Skill Habits on academic procrastination is mediated by Study Self-Efficacy. The SSH construct is specified as a formative latent construct, whereas SSE and procrastination are specified as reflective latent constructs.

  12. Academic Procrastination With © The Author(s) 2022 Classroom

    A narrative review of academic-procrastination inter - ventions (Zacks & Hen, 2018), which also noted that therapeutic prevention and treatment have been com-mon approaches, similarly concluded, "A review of the literature offers scant examples of effective interven-tions for academic procrastination. . . . [M]any studies

  13. Frontiers

    Based on the procrastination literature, we present a selection of such factors and show how they increase the probability of procrastination. ... item 1 "I needlessly delay finishing jobs, even when they're important"). Other academic procrastination scales focus on academic tasks ... is negatively related to procrastination (for review ...

  14. Predictive analysis of college students' academic procrastination

    The predictor of academic procrastination. Extensive literature suggests that researchers are deeply interested in understanding the influencing factors of academic procrastination (Abdi Zarrin ...

  15. What Research Has Been Conducted on Procrastination? Evidence From a

    Regarding research settings, a body of literature has focused on academic procrastination in-depth, and that literature has experienced a significant citation burst (Kim and Seo, 2015; Steel and Klingsieck, 2016). For example, academic procrastination is associated more highly with performance for secondary school students than for other age ...

  16. PDF A Systematic Review of Interventions to Reduce Academic Procrastination

    Academic procrastination is a prevalent and pernicious self-regulation failure, which aects students' academic performance, health, and well-being. We conducted a systematic review of the recent (i.e., 2018 and subsequent) literature on the ecacy of interventions designed to reduce academic procrastination in several relevant online databases.

  17. PDF Academic interventions for academic procrastination: A review of the

    Approaches in decreasing academic procrastination found in the literature can essentially be categorized into three groups; 1. therapeutic treatment, 2. therapeutic prevention, and 3. instructor ...

  18. Academic Procrastination in Children and Adolescents: A Scoping Review

    Abstract. Academic procrastination is a persistent behavior in students' academic development consisting of postponing or delaying the completion of necessary tasks and having a deadline for completion, which is associated with detriment in performance, school dropout, and loss of student well-being. The largest body of existing knowledge on ...

  19. The Influence of Stress Perception on Academic Procrastination in

    Therefore, exploring the factors and internal mechanisms that influence graduate students' academic procrastination is of great relevance to effectively prevent and intervene in their academic procrastination. 2. Literature Review 2.1. Prior Research. Previous studies have shown a strong link between academic procrastination and stress ...

  20. Distance Learning Students' Perspectives of Academic Procrastination: A

    The primary researcher initially formulated a set of interview questions by synthesizing insights from existing literature on academic procrastination. These questions were then aligned with the research's core questions, objectives, and goals. ... Academic interventions for academic procrastination: A review of the literature. Journal of ...

  21. Academic Procrastination of Adolescents: A Brief Review of the Literature

    Procrastination behavior is seen as a problematic behavior both in daily life and academic life (Steel 2007a). AP is a very familiar issue that negatively affects pupils' academic achievement and ...

  22. PDF The Role of Self-Control on Academic Procrastination: A Literature Review

    mpact of self-control on academic procrastination based on prior studies. The review of literature reveals that students' academ. c procrastination is significantly influenced by self-control techniques. The majority of earlier studies determined that there is a negative association between academic procrastination and self-control which ...

  23. Ethics in Patients' Health Literacy: a scoping review and a critical

    The principle of beneficence was issued by Sorensen and Stapleton (Sørensen et al., 2013) in their literature review, arguing that health professionals are ethically responsible for recognizing patients with limited HL and formulating health information according to patients' level. It is the personal liability of the health professional to ...

  24. (PDF) Academic procrastination, family social support and academic

    Findings from the literature review of the academic procrastination, family social support and academic stress on students is there are 20 journals that explain academic stress, academic stress can

  25. Exploring 40 years on affective correlates to procrastination: a

    The relationship between different emotions with situational (e.g., academic) and dispositional (chronic) procrastination was examined extensively in the literature since the early days of procrastination research. A review of empirical studies over the past 40 years might shed light on the role of emotions in procrastination in different contexts with different populations. The current paper ...