• Vygotsky’s Theory of Cognitive Development

Saul Mcleod, PhD

Editor-in-Chief for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MRes, PhD, University of Manchester

Saul Mcleod, PhD., is a qualified psychology teacher with over 18 years of experience in further and higher education. He has been published in peer-reviewed journals, including the Journal of Clinical Psychology.

Learn about our Editorial Process

Olivia Guy-Evans, MSc

Associate Editor for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MSc Psychology of Education

Olivia Guy-Evans is a writer and associate editor for Simply Psychology. She has previously worked in healthcare and educational sectors.

On This Page:

Sociocultural Theory 

The work of Lev Vygotsky (1934, 1978) has become the foundation of much research and theory in cognitive development over the past several decades, particularly what has become known as sociocultural theory.

Vygotsky’s theory comprises concepts such as culture-specific tools, private speech, and the zone of proximal development.

Vygotsky believed cognitive development is influenced by cultural and social factors. He emphasized the role of social interaction in the development of mental abilities e.g., speech and reasoning in children.

Vygotsky strongly believed that community plays a central role in the process of “making meaning.”

Cognitive development is a socially mediated process in which children acquire cultural values, beliefs, and problem-solving strategies through collaborative dialogues with more knowledgeable members of society.

The more knowledgeable other (MKO) is someone who has a higher level of ability or greater understanding than the learner regarding a particular task, process, or concept.

The MKO can be a teacher, parent, coach, or even a peer who provides guidance and modeling to enable the child to learn skills within their zone of proximal development (the gap between what a child can do independently and what they can achieve with guidance).

The interactions with more knowledgeable others significantly increase not only the quantity of information and the number of skills a child develops, but also affects the development of higher-order mental functions such as formal reasoning. Vygotsky argued that higher mental abilities could only develop through interaction with more advanced others.

According to Vygotsky, adults in society foster children’s cognitive development by engaging them in challenging and meaningful activities. Adults convey to children how their culture interprets and responds to the world.

They show the meaning they attach to objects, events, and experiences. They provide the child with what to think (the knowledge) and how to think (the processes, the tools to think with).

Vygotsky’s theory encourages collaborative and cooperative learning between children and teachers or peers. Scaffolding and reciprocal teaching are effective educational strategies based on Vygotsky’s ideas.

Scaffolding involves the teacher providing support structures to help students master skills just beyond their current level. In reciprocal teaching, teachers and students take turns leading discussions using strategies like summarizing and clarifying. Both scaffolding and reciprocal teaching emphasize the shared construction of knowledge, in line with Vygotsky’s views.

Vygotsky highlighted the importance of language in cognitive development. Inner speech is used for mental reasoning, and external speech is used to converse with others.

These operations occur separately. Indeed, before age two, a child employs words socially; they possess no internal language.

Once thought and language merge, however, the social language is internalized and assists the child with their reasoning. Thus, the social environment is ingrained within the child’s learning.

Effects of Culture

Vygotsky emphasized the role of the social environment in the child’s cognitive development.

Vygotsky claimed that infants are born with the basic abilities for intellectual development called “elementary mental functions” (Piaget focuses on motor reflexes and sensory abilities). These develop throughout the first two years of life due to direct environmental contact.

Elementary mental functions include –

o Attention o Sensation o Perception o Memory

Eventually, through interaction within the sociocultural environment, these are developed into more sophisticated and effective mental processes, which Vygotsky refers to as “higher mental functions.”

Tools of intellectual adaptation

Each culture provides its children with tools of intellectual adaptation that allow them to use basic mental functions more effectively/adaptively.

Tools of intellectual adaptation is Vygotsky’s term for methods of thinking and problem-solving strategies that children internalize through social interactions with the more knowledgeable members of society.

For example, memory in young children is limited by biological factors. However, culture determines the type of memory strategy we develop.

For example, in Western culture, children learn note-taking to aid memory, but in pre-literate societies, other strategies must be developed, such as tying knots in a string to remember, carrying pebbles, or repeating the names of ancestors until large numbers can be repeated.

Vygotsky, therefore, sees cognitive functions, even those carried out alone, as affected by the beliefs, values, and tools of intellectual adaptation of the culture in which a person develops and, therefore, socio-culturally determined.

Therefore, intellectual adaptation tools vary from culture to culture – as in the memory example.

Social Influences on Cognitive Development

Like Piaget, Vygotsky believes that young children are curious and actively involved in their own learning and discovering and developing new understandings/schema .

However, Vygotsky emphasized social contributions to the development process, whereas Piaget emphasized self-initiated discovery.

According to Vygotsky (1978), much important learning by the child occurs through social interaction with a skillful tutor. The tutor may model behaviors and/or provide verbal instructions for the child.

Vygotsky refers to this as cooperative or collaborative dialogue. The child seeks to understand the actions or instructions provided by the tutor (often the parent or teacher) and then internalizes the information, using it to guide or regulate their performance.

Shaffer (1996) gives the example of a young girl given her first jigsaw. Alone, she performs poorly in attempting to solve the puzzle. The father then sits with her and describes or demonstrates some basic strategies, such as finding all the corner/edge pieces, and provides a couple of pieces for the child to put together herself, and offers encouragement when she does so.

As the child becomes more competent, the father allows the child to work more independently. According to Vygotsky, this social interaction involving cooperative or collaborative dialogue promotes cognitive development.

To understand Vygotsky’s theories on cognitive development, one must understand two of the main principles of Vygotsky’s work: the More Knowledgeable Other (MKO) and the Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD).

More Knowledgeable Other

The more knowledgeable other (MKO) is somewhat self-explanatory; it refers to someone who has a better understanding or a higher ability level than the learner, concerning a particular task, process, or concept.

Although the implication is that the MKO is a teacher or an older adult, this is not necessarily the case. Often, a child’s peers or an adult’s children may be the individuals with more knowledge or experience.

For example, who is more likely to know more about the newest teenage music groups, how to win at the most recent PlayStation game, or how to correctly perform the newest dance craze – a child or their parents?

In fact, the MKO need not be a person at all. To support employees in their learning process, some companies are now using electronic performance support systems.

Electronic tutors have also been used in educational settings to facilitate and guide students through learning. The key to MKOs is that they must have (or be programmed with) more knowledge about the topic being learned than the learner does.

Zone of Proximal Development

The concept of the more knowledgeable other relates to the second important principle of Vygotsky’s work, the zone of proximal development .

This important concept relates to the difference between what a child can achieve independently and what a child can achieve with guidance and encouragement from a skilled partner.

Vygotsky consequently focuses much more closely on social interaction as an aid to learning, arguing that, left alone, children will develop – but not to their full potential.

He refers to the gap between actual and potential learning as the zone of proximal development (ZPD) – and argues that it is only through collaboration with adults and other learners that this gap can be bridged.

Vygotsky

The zone of proximal development is the gap between the level of actual development, what the child can do on his own, and the level of potential development, what a child can do with the assistance of more advanced and competent individuals.

Social interaction, therefore, supports the child’s cognitive development in the ZPD, leading to a higher level of reasoning. It is generally believed that social dialogues have two important features.

The first is intersubjectivity, where two individuals who might have different understandings of a task, arrive at a shared understanding by adjusting to the perspective of the other.

The second feature is referred to as scaffolding. Adults may begin with direct instruction, but as children’s mastery of a task increases, so the adult tends to withdraw their own contributions in recognition of the child’s increasing success.

For example, the child could not solve the jigsaw puzzle (in the example above) by itself and would have taken a long time to do so (if at all), but was able to solve it following interaction with the father, and has developed competence at this skill that will be applied to future jigsaws.

ZPD is the zone where instruction is the most beneficial, as it is when the task is just beyond the individual’s capabilities. To learn, we must be presented with tasks just out of our ability range. Challenging tasks promote maximum cognitive growth.

As a result of shared dialogues with more knowledgeable others, who provide hints, instructions, and encouragement, the child can internalize the ‘how to do it’ part of the task as part of their inner or private speech. The child can then use this on later occasions when they tackle a similar task on their own.

Vygotsky (1978) sees the Zone of Proximal Development as the area where the most sensitive instruction or guidance should be given – allowing the child to develop skills they will then use on their own – developing higher mental functions.

Vygotsky also views peer interaction as an effective way of developing skills and strategies.  He suggests that teachers use cooperative learning exercises where less competent children develop with help from more skillful peers – within the zone of proximal development.

Evidence for Vygotsky and the ZPD

Freund (1990) conducted a study in which children had to decide which items of furniture should be placed in particular areas of a doll’s house.

Some children were allowed to play with their mother in a similar situation before they attempted it alone (zone of proximal development) while others were allowed to work on this by themselves (Piaget’s discovery learning).

Freund found that those who had previously worked with their mother (ZPD) showed the greatest improvement compared with their first attempt at the task.

The conclusion is that guided learning within the ZPD led to greater understanding/performance than working alone (discovery learning).

Vygotsky and Language

Vygotsky believed that language develops from social interactions for communication purposes. Vygotsky viewed language as man’s greatest tool for communicating with the outside world.

According to Vygotsky (1962), language plays two critical roles in cognitive development:
  • It is the main means by which adults transmit information to children.
  • Language itself becomes a very powerful tool for intellectual adaptation.
Vygotsky (1987) differentiates between three forms of language:
  • Social speech, which is external communication used to talk to others (typical from the age of two);
  • Private speech (typical from the age of three) which is directed to the self and serves an intellectual function;
  • Private speech goes underground , diminishing in audibility as it takes on a self-regulating function and is transformed into silent inner speech (typical from the age of seven).

For Vygotsky, thought and language are initially separate systems from the beginning of life, merging at around three years of age.

At this point, speech and thought become interdependent: thought becomes verbal, and speech becomes representational.

As children develop mental representation, particularly the skill of language, they start to communicate with themselves in much the same way as they would communicate with others.

When this happens, children’s monologues are internalized to become inner speech. The internalization of language is important as it drives cognitive development.

“Inner speech is not the interiour aspect of external speech – it is a function in itself. It still remains speech, i.e., thought connected with words. But while in external speech thought is embodied in words, in inner speech words dies as they bring forth thought. Inner speech is to a large extent thinking in pure meanings.” (Vygotsky, 1962: p. 149)

Private Speech

Vygotsky (1987) was the first psychologist to document the importance of private speech.

He considered private speech as the transition point between social and inner speech, the moment in development where language and thought unite to constitute verbal thinking.

Thus, in Vygotsky’s view, private speech was the earliest manifestation of inner speech. Indeed, private speech is more similar (in form and function) to inner speech than social speech.

Private speech is “typically defined, in contrast to social speech, as speech addressed to the self (not to others) for the purpose of self-regulation (rather than communication).” (Diaz, 1992, p.62)

Private speech is overt, audible, and observable, often seen in children who talk to themselves while problem-solving.

Conversely, inner speech is covert or hidden because it happens internally. It is the silent, internal dialogue that adults often engage in while thinking or problem-solving.

In contrast to Piaget’s (1959) notion of private speech representing a developmental dead-end, Vygotsky (1934, 1987) viewed private speech as:

“A revolution in development which is triggered when preverbal thought and preintellectual language come together to create fundamentally new forms of mental functioning.” (Fernyhough & Fradley, 2005: p. 1)

In addition to disagreeing on the functional significance of private speech, Vygotsky and Piaget also offered opposing views on the developmental course of private speech and the environmental circumstances in which it occurs most often (Berk & Garvin, 1984).

Piaget

Through private speech, children collaborate with themselves, in the same way a more knowledgeable other (e.g., adults) collaborate with them to achieve a given function.

Vygotsky sees “private speech” as a means for children to plan activities and strategies, aiding their development. Private speech is the use of language for self-regulation of behavior.

Therefore, language accelerates thinking/understanding ( Jerome Bruner also views language in this way). Vygotsky believed that children who engage in large amounts of private speech are more socially competent than children who do not use it extensively.

Vygotsky (1987) notes that private speech does not merely accompany a child’s activity but acts as a tool the developing child uses to facilitate cognitive processes, such as overcoming task obstacles, and enhancing imagination, thinking, and conscious awareness.

Children use private speech most often during intermediate difficulty tasks because they attempt to self-regulate by verbally planning and organizing their thoughts (Winsler et al., 2007).

The frequency and content of private speech correlate with behavior or performance. For example, private speech appears functionally related to cognitive performance: It appears at times of difficulty with a task.

For example, tasks related to executive function (Fernyhough & Fradley, 2005), problem-solving tasks (Behrend et al., 1992), and schoolwork in both language (Berk & Landau, 1993), and mathematics (Ostad & Sorensen, 2007).

Berk (1986) provided empirical support for the notion of private speech. She found that most private speech exhibited by children serves to describe or guide the child’s actions.

Berk also discovered that children engaged in private speech more often when working alone on challenging tasks and when their teacher was not immediately available to help them.

Furthermore, Berk also found that private speech develops similarly in all children regardless of cultural background.

There is also evidence (Behrend et al., 1992) that those children who displayed the characteristic whispering and lip movements associated with private speech when faced with a difficult task were generally more attentive and successful than their ‘quieter’ classmates.

Vygotsky (1987) proposed that private speech is a product of an individual’s social environment. This hypothesis is supported by the fact that there exist high positive correlations between rates of social interaction and private speech in children.

Children raised in cognitively and linguistically stimulating environments (situations more frequently observed in higher socioeconomic status families) start using and internalizing private speech faster than children from less privileged backgrounds.

Indeed, children raised in environments characterized by low verbal and social exchanges exhibit delays in private speech development.

Children’s use of private speech diminishes as they grow older and follows a curvilinear trend. This is due to changes in ontogenetic development whereby children can internalize language (through inner speech) to self-regulate their behavior (Vygotsky, 1987).

For example, research has shown that children’s private speech usually peaks at 3–4 years of age, decreases at 6–7, and gradually fades out to be mostly internalized by age 10 (Diaz, 1992).

Vygotsky proposed that private speech diminishes and disappears with age not because it becomes socialized, as Piaget suggested, but because it goes underground to constitute inner speech or verbal thought” (Frauenglass & Diaz, 1985).

Educational Implications

Vygotsky’s approach to child development is a form of social constructivism , based on the idea that cognitive functions are the products of social interactions.

Social constructivism posits that knowledge is constructed and learning occurs through social interactions within a cultural and historical context.

Vygotsky emphasized the collaborative nature of learning by constructing knowledge through social negotiation. He rejected the assumption made by Piaget that it was possible to separate learning from its social context.

Vygotsky believed everything is learned on two levels. First, through interaction with others, then integrated into the individual’s mental structure.

Every function in the child’s cultural development appears twice: first, on the social level, and later, on the individual level; first, between people (interpsychological) and then inside the child (intrapsychological). This applies equally to voluntary attention, to logical memory, and to the formation of concepts. All the higher functions originate as actual relationships between individuals. (Vygotsky, 1978, p.57)

Teaching styles grounded in constructivism represent a deliberate shift from traditional, didactic, memory-oriented transmission models (Cannella & Reiff, 1994) to a more student-centered approach.

Traditionally, schools have failed to foster environments where students actively participate in their own and their peers’ education. Vygotsky’s theory, however, calls for both the teacher and students to assume non-traditional roles as they engage in collaborative learning.

Rather than having a teacher impose their understanding onto students for future recitation, the teacher should co-create meaning with students in a manner that allows learners to take ownership (Hausfather, 1996).

For instance, a student and teacher might start a task with varying levels of expertise and understanding. As they adapt to each other’s perspective, the teacher must articulate their insights in a way that the student can comprehend, leading the student to a fuller understanding of the task or concept.

The student can then internalize the task’s operational aspect (“how to do it”) into their inner speech or private dialogue. Vygotsky referred to this reciprocal understanding and adjustment process as intersubjectivity.”

Because Vygotsky asserts that cognitive change occurs within the zone of proximal development, instruction would be designed to reach a developmental level just above the student’s current developmental level.

Vygotsky proclaims, “learning which is oriented toward developmental levels that have already been reached is ineffective from the viewpoint of the child’s overall development. It does not aim for a new stage of the developmental process but rather lags behind this process” (Vygotsky, 1978).

Appropriation is necessary for cognitive development within the zone of proximal development. Individuals participating in peer collaboration or guided teacher instruction must share the same focus to access the zone of proximal development.

“Joint attention and shared problem solving is needed to create a process of cognitive, social, and emotional interchange” (Hausfather,1996).

Furthermore, it is essential that the partners be on different developmental levels and the higher-level partner be aware of the lower’s level. If this does not occur or one partner dominates, the interaction is less successful (Driscoll, 1994; Hausfather, 1996).

Vygotsky’s theories also feed into the current interest in collaborative learning, suggesting that group members should have different levels of ability so more advanced peers can help less advanced members operate within their ZPD.

Scaffolding and reciprocal teaching are effective strategies to access the zone of proximal development.

Reciprocal Teaching

A contemporary educational application of Vygotsky’s theory is “reciprocal teaching,” used to improve students” ability to learn from text.

In this method, teachers and students collaborate in learning and practicing four key skills: summarizing, questioning, clarifying, and predicting. The teacher’s role in the process is reduced over time.

Reciprocal teaching allows for the creation of a dialogue between students and teachers. This two-way communication becomes an instructional strategy by encouraging students to go beyond answering questions and engage in the discourse (Driscoll, 1994; Hausfather, 1996).

A study conducted by Brown and Palincsar (1989) demonstrated the Vygotskian approach with reciprocal teaching methods in their successful program to teach reading strategies.

The teacher and students alternated turns leading small group discussions on a reading. After modeling four reading strategies, students began to assume the teaching role.

The results showed significant gains over other instructional strategies (Driscoll, 1994; Hausfather,1996).

Cognitively Guided Instruction is another strategy to implement Vygotsky’s theory. This strategy involves the teacher and students exploring math problems and then sharing their problem-solving strategies in an open dialogue (Hausfather,1996).

Based on Vygotsky’s theory, the physical classroom would provide clustered desks or tables and workspace for peer instruction, collaboration, and small-group instruction. Learning becomes a reciprocal experience for the students and teacher.

Like the environment, the instructional design of the material to be learned would be structured to promote and encourage student interaction and collaboration. Thus the classroom becomes a community of learning.

Scaffolding

Also, Vygotsky’s theory of cognitive development on learners is relevant to instructional concepts such as “scaffolding” and “apprenticeship,” in which a teacher or more advanced peer helps to structure or arrange a task so that a novice can work on it successfully.

A teacher’s role is to identify each individual’s current level of development and provide them with opportunities to cross their ZPD.

A crucial element in this process is the use of what later became known as scaffolding; the way in which the teacher provides students with frameworks and experiences which encourage them to extend their existing schemata and incorporate new skills, competencies, and understandings.

Scaffolding describes the conditions that support the child’s learning, to move from what they already know to new knowledge and abilities.

Scaffolding requires the teacher to allow students to extend their current skills and knowledge.

During scaffolding, the support offered by an adult (or more knowledgeable other) gradually decreases as the child becomes more skilled in the task.

As the adult withdraws their help, the child assumes more of the strategic planning and eventually gains competence to master similar problems without a teacher’s aid or a more knowledgeable peer.

It is important to note that this is more than simply instruction; learning experiences must be presented in such a way as to actively challenge existing mental structures and provide frameworks for learning.

Five ways in which an adult can “scaffold” a child’s learning:

  • Engaging the child’s interest
  • Maintaining the child’s interest in the task e.g., avoiding distraction and providing clear instructions on how to start the task.
  • Keeping the child’s frustration under control e.g., by supportive interactions, adapting instructions according to where the child is struggling.
  • Emphasizing the important features of the task
  • Demonstrating the task: showing the child how to do the task in simple, clear steps.

As the child progresses through the ZPD, the necessary scaffolding level declines from 5 to 1.

The teacher must engage students’ interests, simplify tasks to be manageable, and motivate students to pursue the instructional goal.

In addition, the teacher must look for discrepancies between students” efforts and the solution, control for frustration and risk, and model an idealized version of the act (Hausfather, 1996).

Challenges to Traditional Teaching Methods

Vygotsky’s social development theory challenges traditional teaching methods. Historically, schools have been organized around recitation teaching.

The teacher disseminates knowledge to be memorized by the students, who in turn recite the information to the teacher (Hausfather,1996).

However, the studies described above offer empirical evidence that learning based on the social development theory facilitates cognitive development over other instructional strategies.

The structure of our schools does not reflect the rapid changes our society is experiencing. The introduction and integration of computer technology in society has tremendously increased the opportunities for social interaction.

Therefore, the social context for learning is transforming as well. Whereas collaboration and peer instruction were once only possible in shared physical space, learning relationships can now be formed from distances through cyberspace.

Computer technology is a cultural tool that students can use to meditate and internalize their learning. Recent research suggests changing the learning contexts with technology is a powerful learning activity (Crawford, 1996).

If schools continue to resist structural change, students will be ill-prepared for the world they will live.

Critical Evaluation

Vygotsky’s work has not received the same level of intense scrutiny that Piaget’s has, partly due to the time-consuming process of translating Vygotsky’s work from Russian.

Also, Vygotsky’s sociocultural perspective does not provide as many specific hypotheses to test as Piaget’s theory, making refutation difficult, if not impossible.

Perhaps the main criticism of Vygotsky’s work concerns the assumption that it is relevant to all cultures. Rogoff (1990) dismisses the idea that Vygotsky’s ideas are culturally universal and instead states that scaffolding- heavily dependent on verbal instruction – may not be equally useful in all cultures for all types of learning.

Indeed, in some instances, observation and practice may be more effective ways of learning certain skills.

There is much emphasis on social interaction and culture, but many other aspects of development are neglected, such as the importance of emotional factors, e.g., the joys of success and the disappointments and frustration of failure act as motivation for learning.

Vygotsky overemphasized socio-cultural factors at the expense of biological influences on cognitive development. This theory cannot explain why cross-cultural studies show that the stages of development (except the formal operational stage ) occur in the same order in all cultures suggesting that cognitive development is a product of a biological process of maturation.

Vygotky’s theory has been applied successfully to education. Scaffolding has been shown to be an effective way of teaching (Freund, 1990), and based on this theory, teachers are trained to guide children from what they can do to the next step in their learning through careful scaffolding.

Collaborative work is also used in the classroom, mixing children of different levels of ability to make use of reciprocal / peer teaching.

Vygotsky vs. Piaget

Unlike Piaget’s notion that children’s cognitive development must necessarily precede their learning, Vygotsky argued, “learning is a necessary and universal aspect of the process of developing culturally organized, specifically human psychological function” (1978, p. 90).  In other words, social learning precedes (i.e., come before) development.

Differences betwee Vygotsky and Piaget In Psychology

Vygotsky’s theory differs from that of Piaget in several important ways:

Vygotsky places more emphasis on culture affecting cognitive development.

Unlike Piaget, who emphasized universal cognitive change (i.e., all children would go through the same sequence of cognitive development regardless of their cultural experiences), Vygotsky leads us to expect variable development depending on cultural diversity. 

This contradicts Piaget’s view of universal stages of development (Vygotsky does not refer to stages like Piaget does).

Hence, Vygotsky assumes cognitive development varies across cultures, whereas Piaget states cognitive development is mostly universal across cultures.

Vygotsky places considerably more emphasis on social factors contributing to cognitive development.

  • Vygotsky states the importance of cultural and social context for learning. Cognitive development stems from social interactions from guided learning within the zone of proximal development as children and their partners co-construct knowledge. In contrast, Piaget maintains that cognitive development stems largely from independent explorations in which children construct knowledge.
  • For Vygotsky, the environment in which children grow up will influence how they think and what they think about. The importance of scaffolding and language may differ for all cultures. Rogoff (1990) emphasizes the importance of observation and practice in pre-industrial societies (e.g., learning to use a canoe among Micronesian Islanders).

Vygotsky places more (and different) emphasis on the role of language in cognitive development.

According to Piaget , language depends on thought for its development (i.e., thought comes before language). For Vygotsky, thought and language are initially separate systems from the beginning of life, merging at around three years of age, producing verbal thought (inner speech).

In Piaget’s theory, egocentric (or private) speech gradually disappears as children develop truly social speech, in which they monitor and adapt what they say to others.

Vygotsky disagreed with this view, arguing that as language helps children to think about and control their behavior, it is an important foundation for complex cognitive skills.

As children age, this self-directed speech becomes silent (or private) speech, referring to the inner dialogues we have with ourselves as we plan and carry out activities.

For Vygotsky, cognitive development results from an internalization of language.

According to Vygotsky, adults are an important source of cognitive development.

Adults transmit their culture’s tools of intellectual adaptation that children internalize.

In contrast, Piaget emphasizes the importance of peers, as peer interaction promotes social perspective-taking.

Behrend, D.A., Rosengren, K.S., & Perlmutter, M. (1992). The relation between private speech and parental interactive style. In R.M. Diaz & L.E. Berk (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 85–100) . Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Berk, L. E. (1986). Relationship of elementary school children’s private speech to behavioral accompaniment to task, attention, and task performance. Developmental Psychology, 22(5) , 671.

Berk, L. & Garvin, R. (1984). Development of private speech among low-income Appalachian children. Developmental Psychology, 20(2) , 271-286.

Berk, L. E., & Landau, S. (1993). Private speech of learning-disabled and normally achieving children in classroom academic and laboratory contexts. Child Development, 64 , 556–571.

Cannella, G. S., & Reiff, J. C. (1994). Individual constructivist teacher education: Teachers as empowered learners . Teacher education quarterly , 27-38.

Crawford, K. (1996) Vygotskian approaches to human development in the information era. Educational Studies in Mathematics, (31) ,43-62.

Diaz, R. M., & Berk, L. E. (1992). Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation. Lawrence Erlbaum.

Driscoll, M. P. (1994). Psychology of Learning for Instruction . Needham, Ma: Allyn && Bacon.

Frauenglass, M. & Diaz, R. (1985). Self-regulatory functions of children’s private speech: A critical analysis of recent challenges to Vygotsky’s theory. Developmental Psychology, 21(2) , 357-364.

Fernyhough, C., & Fradley, E. (2005). Private speech on an executive task: Relations with task difficulty and task performance . Cognitive Development, 20 , 103–120.

Freund, L. S. (1990). Maternal regulation of children’s problem-solving behavior and its impact on children’s performance . Child Development, 61 , 113-126.

Hausfather, S. J. (1996). Vygotsky and Schooling: Creating a Social Contest for learning. Action in Teacher Education, (18) ,1-10.

Ostad, S. A., & Sorensen, P. M. (2007). Private speech and strategy-use patterns: Bidirectional comparisons of children with and without mathematical difficulties in a developmental perspective. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40 , 2–14.

Piaget, J. (1959). The language and thought of the child (Vol. 5) . Psychology Press.

Rogoff, B. (1990).  Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context . Oxford university press.

Saettler, P. (1990). The Evolution of American Educational Technology . Egnlewood, Co: Libraries Unlimited.

Schaffer, R. (1996) . Social development. Oxford: Blackwell.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Thinking and speech. In R.W. Rieber & A.S. Carton (Eds.), The collected works of L.S. Vygotsky, Volume 1: Problems of general psychology (pp. 39–285) . New York: Plenum Press. (Original work published 1934.)

Winsler, A., Abar, B., Feder, M. A., Schunn, C. D., & Rubio, D. A. (2007). Private speech and executive functioning among high-functioning children with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37 , 1617-1635.

Wertsch, J. V., Sohmer, R. (1995). Vygotsky on learning and development. Human Development, (38), 332-37.

Further Reading

What is vygotsky’s theory.

Vygotsky believed that cognitive development was founded on social interaction. According to Vygotsky, much of what children acquire in their understanding of the world is the product of collaboration.

How is Vygotsky’s theory applied in teaching and learning?

Vygotsky’s theory has profound implications for classroom learning. Teachers guide, support, and encourage children, yet also help them to develop problem-solving strategies that can be generalized to other situations.

Children learn best not when they are isolated, but when they interact with others, particularly more knowledgeable others who can provide the guidance and encouragement to master new skills.

What was Vygotsky’s best know concept?

Lev Vygotsky was a seminal Russian psychologist best known for his sociocultural theory. He constructed the idea of a zone of proximal development ,  which are those tasks which are too difficult for a child to solve alone but s/he can accomplish with the help of adults or more skilled peers.

Vygotsky has developed a sociocultural approach to cognitive development. He developed his theories at around the same time as  Jean Piaget  was starting to develop his ideas (1920’s and 30″s), but he died at the age of 38, and so his theories are incomplete – although some of his writings are still being translated from Russian.

Like Piaget, Vygotsky could be described as a  constructivist , in that he was interested in knowledge acquisition as a cumulative event – with new experiences and understandings incorporated into existing cognitive frameworks.

However, while Piaget’s theory is structural (arguing that physiological stages govern development), Vygotsky denies the existence of any guiding framework independent of culture and context.

No single principle (such as Piaget’s equilibration) can account for development. Individual development cannot be understood without reference to the social and cultural context within which it is embedded. Higher mental processes in the individual have their origin in social processes.

What is Vygotsky’s Social Development Theory?

Vygotsky’s Social Development Theory is often referred to as the Sociocultural Theory.

Vygotsky’s Social Development Theory posits that social interaction is fundamental to cognitive development. Vygotsky emphasized the influence of cultural and social contexts on learning, claiming that knowledge is constructed through social collaboration.

His most known concept, the Zone of Proximal Development, refers to the difference between what a learner can do independently and what they can achieve with guidance.

Print Friendly, PDF & Email

Related Articles

Vygotsky vs. Piaget: A Paradigm Shift

Child Psychology

Vygotsky vs. Piaget: A Paradigm Shift

Interactional Synchrony

Interactional Synchrony

Internal Working Models of Attachment

Internal Working Models of Attachment

Soft Determinism In Psychology

Soft Determinism In Psychology

Branches of Psychology

Branches of Psychology

Learning Theory of Attachment

Learning Theory of Attachment

  • Search Menu
  • Browse content in Arts and Humanities
  • Browse content in Archaeology
  • Anglo-Saxon and Medieval Archaeology
  • Archaeological Methodology and Techniques
  • Archaeology by Region
  • Archaeology of Religion
  • Archaeology of Trade and Exchange
  • Biblical Archaeology
  • Contemporary and Public Archaeology
  • Environmental Archaeology
  • Historical Archaeology
  • History and Theory of Archaeology
  • Industrial Archaeology
  • Landscape Archaeology
  • Mortuary Archaeology
  • Prehistoric Archaeology
  • Underwater Archaeology
  • Urban Archaeology
  • Zooarchaeology
  • Browse content in Architecture
  • Architectural Structure and Design
  • History of Architecture
  • Residential and Domestic Buildings
  • Theory of Architecture
  • Browse content in Art
  • Art Subjects and Themes
  • History of Art
  • Industrial and Commercial Art
  • Theory of Art
  • Biographical Studies
  • Byzantine Studies
  • Browse content in Classical Studies
  • Classical History
  • Classical Philosophy
  • Classical Mythology
  • Classical Literature
  • Classical Reception
  • Classical Art and Architecture
  • Classical Oratory and Rhetoric
  • Greek and Roman Papyrology
  • Greek and Roman Epigraphy
  • Greek and Roman Law
  • Greek and Roman Archaeology
  • Late Antiquity
  • Religion in the Ancient World
  • Digital Humanities
  • Browse content in History
  • Colonialism and Imperialism
  • Diplomatic History
  • Environmental History
  • Genealogy, Heraldry, Names, and Honours
  • Genocide and Ethnic Cleansing
  • Historical Geography
  • History by Period
  • History of Emotions
  • History of Agriculture
  • History of Education
  • History of Gender and Sexuality
  • Industrial History
  • Intellectual History
  • International History
  • Labour History
  • Legal and Constitutional History
  • Local and Family History
  • Maritime History
  • Military History
  • National Liberation and Post-Colonialism
  • Oral History
  • Political History
  • Public History
  • Regional and National History
  • Revolutions and Rebellions
  • Slavery and Abolition of Slavery
  • Social and Cultural History
  • Theory, Methods, and Historiography
  • Urban History
  • World History
  • Browse content in Language Teaching and Learning
  • Language Learning (Specific Skills)
  • Language Teaching Theory and Methods
  • Browse content in Linguistics
  • Applied Linguistics
  • Cognitive Linguistics
  • Computational Linguistics
  • Forensic Linguistics
  • Grammar, Syntax and Morphology
  • Historical and Diachronic Linguistics
  • History of English
  • Language Evolution
  • Language Reference
  • Language Acquisition
  • Language Variation
  • Language Families
  • Lexicography
  • Linguistic Anthropology
  • Linguistic Theories
  • Linguistic Typology
  • Phonetics and Phonology
  • Psycholinguistics
  • Sociolinguistics
  • Translation and Interpretation
  • Writing Systems
  • Browse content in Literature
  • Bibliography
  • Children's Literature Studies
  • Literary Studies (Romanticism)
  • Literary Studies (American)
  • Literary Studies (Asian)
  • Literary Studies (European)
  • Literary Studies (Eco-criticism)
  • Literary Studies (Modernism)
  • Literary Studies - World
  • Literary Studies (1500 to 1800)
  • Literary Studies (19th Century)
  • Literary Studies (20th Century onwards)
  • Literary Studies (African American Literature)
  • Literary Studies (British and Irish)
  • Literary Studies (Early and Medieval)
  • Literary Studies (Fiction, Novelists, and Prose Writers)
  • Literary Studies (Gender Studies)
  • Literary Studies (Graphic Novels)
  • Literary Studies (History of the Book)
  • Literary Studies (Plays and Playwrights)
  • Literary Studies (Poetry and Poets)
  • Literary Studies (Postcolonial Literature)
  • Literary Studies (Queer Studies)
  • Literary Studies (Science Fiction)
  • Literary Studies (Travel Literature)
  • Literary Studies (War Literature)
  • Literary Studies (Women's Writing)
  • Literary Theory and Cultural Studies
  • Mythology and Folklore
  • Shakespeare Studies and Criticism
  • Browse content in Media Studies
  • Browse content in Music
  • Applied Music
  • Dance and Music
  • Ethics in Music
  • Ethnomusicology
  • Gender and Sexuality in Music
  • Medicine and Music
  • Music Cultures
  • Music and Media
  • Music and Religion
  • Music and Culture
  • Music Education and Pedagogy
  • Music Theory and Analysis
  • Musical Scores, Lyrics, and Libretti
  • Musical Structures, Styles, and Techniques
  • Musicology and Music History
  • Performance Practice and Studies
  • Race and Ethnicity in Music
  • Sound Studies
  • Browse content in Performing Arts
  • Browse content in Philosophy
  • Aesthetics and Philosophy of Art
  • Epistemology
  • Feminist Philosophy
  • History of Western Philosophy
  • Metaphysics
  • Moral Philosophy
  • Non-Western Philosophy
  • Philosophy of Language
  • Philosophy of Mind
  • Philosophy of Perception
  • Philosophy of Science
  • Philosophy of Action
  • Philosophy of Law
  • Philosophy of Religion
  • Philosophy of Mathematics and Logic
  • Practical Ethics
  • Social and Political Philosophy
  • Browse content in Religion
  • Biblical Studies
  • Christianity
  • East Asian Religions
  • History of Religion
  • Judaism and Jewish Studies
  • Qumran Studies
  • Religion and Education
  • Religion and Health
  • Religion and Politics
  • Religion and Science
  • Religion and Law
  • Religion and Art, Literature, and Music
  • Religious Studies
  • Browse content in Society and Culture
  • Cookery, Food, and Drink
  • Cultural Studies
  • Customs and Traditions
  • Ethical Issues and Debates
  • Hobbies, Games, Arts and Crafts
  • Lifestyle, Home, and Garden
  • Natural world, Country Life, and Pets
  • Popular Beliefs and Controversial Knowledge
  • Sports and Outdoor Recreation
  • Technology and Society
  • Travel and Holiday
  • Visual Culture
  • Browse content in Law
  • Arbitration
  • Browse content in Company and Commercial Law
  • Commercial Law
  • Company Law
  • Browse content in Comparative Law
  • Systems of Law
  • Competition Law
  • Browse content in Constitutional and Administrative Law
  • Government Powers
  • Judicial Review
  • Local Government Law
  • Military and Defence Law
  • Parliamentary and Legislative Practice
  • Construction Law
  • Contract Law
  • Browse content in Criminal Law
  • Criminal Procedure
  • Criminal Evidence Law
  • Sentencing and Punishment
  • Employment and Labour Law
  • Environment and Energy Law
  • Browse content in Financial Law
  • Banking Law
  • Insolvency Law
  • History of Law
  • Human Rights and Immigration
  • Intellectual Property Law
  • Browse content in International Law
  • Private International Law and Conflict of Laws
  • Public International Law
  • IT and Communications Law
  • Jurisprudence and Philosophy of Law
  • Law and Politics
  • Law and Society
  • Browse content in Legal System and Practice
  • Courts and Procedure
  • Legal Skills and Practice
  • Primary Sources of Law
  • Regulation of Legal Profession
  • Medical and Healthcare Law
  • Browse content in Policing
  • Criminal Investigation and Detection
  • Police and Security Services
  • Police Procedure and Law
  • Police Regional Planning
  • Browse content in Property Law
  • Personal Property Law
  • Study and Revision
  • Terrorism and National Security Law
  • Browse content in Trusts Law
  • Wills and Probate or Succession
  • Browse content in Medicine and Health
  • Browse content in Allied Health Professions
  • Arts Therapies
  • Clinical Science
  • Dietetics and Nutrition
  • Occupational Therapy
  • Operating Department Practice
  • Physiotherapy
  • Radiography
  • Speech and Language Therapy
  • Browse content in Anaesthetics
  • General Anaesthesia
  • Neuroanaesthesia
  • Clinical Neuroscience
  • Browse content in Clinical Medicine
  • Acute Medicine
  • Cardiovascular Medicine
  • Clinical Genetics
  • Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics
  • Dermatology
  • Endocrinology and Diabetes
  • Gastroenterology
  • Genito-urinary Medicine
  • Geriatric Medicine
  • Infectious Diseases
  • Medical Toxicology
  • Medical Oncology
  • Pain Medicine
  • Palliative Medicine
  • Rehabilitation Medicine
  • Respiratory Medicine and Pulmonology
  • Rheumatology
  • Sleep Medicine
  • Sports and Exercise Medicine
  • Community Medical Services
  • Critical Care
  • Emergency Medicine
  • Forensic Medicine
  • Haematology
  • History of Medicine
  • Browse content in Medical Skills
  • Clinical Skills
  • Communication Skills
  • Nursing Skills
  • Surgical Skills
  • Browse content in Medical Dentistry
  • Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery
  • Paediatric Dentistry
  • Restorative Dentistry and Orthodontics
  • Surgical Dentistry
  • Medical Ethics
  • Medical Statistics and Methodology
  • Browse content in Neurology
  • Clinical Neurophysiology
  • Neuropathology
  • Nursing Studies
  • Browse content in Obstetrics and Gynaecology
  • Gynaecology
  • Occupational Medicine
  • Ophthalmology
  • Otolaryngology (ENT)
  • Browse content in Paediatrics
  • Neonatology
  • Browse content in Pathology
  • Chemical Pathology
  • Clinical Cytogenetics and Molecular Genetics
  • Histopathology
  • Medical Microbiology and Virology
  • Patient Education and Information
  • Browse content in Pharmacology
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Browse content in Popular Health
  • Caring for Others
  • Complementary and Alternative Medicine
  • Self-help and Personal Development
  • Browse content in Preclinical Medicine
  • Cell Biology
  • Molecular Biology and Genetics
  • Reproduction, Growth and Development
  • Primary Care
  • Professional Development in Medicine
  • Browse content in Psychiatry
  • Addiction Medicine
  • Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
  • Forensic Psychiatry
  • Learning Disabilities
  • Old Age Psychiatry
  • Psychotherapy
  • Browse content in Public Health and Epidemiology
  • Epidemiology
  • Public Health
  • Browse content in Radiology
  • Clinical Radiology
  • Interventional Radiology
  • Nuclear Medicine
  • Radiation Oncology
  • Reproductive Medicine
  • Browse content in Surgery
  • Cardiothoracic Surgery
  • Gastro-intestinal and Colorectal Surgery
  • General Surgery
  • Neurosurgery
  • Paediatric Surgery
  • Peri-operative Care
  • Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery
  • Surgical Oncology
  • Transplant Surgery
  • Trauma and Orthopaedic Surgery
  • Vascular Surgery
  • Browse content in Science and Mathematics
  • Browse content in Biological Sciences
  • Aquatic Biology
  • Biochemistry
  • Bioinformatics and Computational Biology
  • Developmental Biology
  • Ecology and Conservation
  • Evolutionary Biology
  • Genetics and Genomics
  • Microbiology
  • Molecular and Cell Biology
  • Natural History
  • Plant Sciences and Forestry
  • Research Methods in Life Sciences
  • Structural Biology
  • Systems Biology
  • Zoology and Animal Sciences
  • Browse content in Chemistry
  • Analytical Chemistry
  • Computational Chemistry
  • Crystallography
  • Environmental Chemistry
  • Industrial Chemistry
  • Inorganic Chemistry
  • Materials Chemistry
  • Medicinal Chemistry
  • Mineralogy and Gems
  • Organic Chemistry
  • Physical Chemistry
  • Polymer Chemistry
  • Study and Communication Skills in Chemistry
  • Theoretical Chemistry
  • Browse content in Computer Science
  • Artificial Intelligence
  • Computer Architecture and Logic Design
  • Game Studies
  • Human-Computer Interaction
  • Mathematical Theory of Computation
  • Programming Languages
  • Software Engineering
  • Systems Analysis and Design
  • Virtual Reality
  • Browse content in Computing
  • Business Applications
  • Computer Security
  • Computer Games
  • Computer Networking and Communications
  • Digital Lifestyle
  • Graphical and Digital Media Applications
  • Operating Systems
  • Browse content in Earth Sciences and Geography
  • Atmospheric Sciences
  • Environmental Geography
  • Geology and the Lithosphere
  • Maps and Map-making
  • Meteorology and Climatology
  • Oceanography and Hydrology
  • Palaeontology
  • Physical Geography and Topography
  • Regional Geography
  • Soil Science
  • Urban Geography
  • Browse content in Engineering and Technology
  • Agriculture and Farming
  • Biological Engineering
  • Civil Engineering, Surveying, and Building
  • Electronics and Communications Engineering
  • Energy Technology
  • Engineering (General)
  • Environmental Science, Engineering, and Technology
  • History of Engineering and Technology
  • Mechanical Engineering and Materials
  • Technology of Industrial Chemistry
  • Transport Technology and Trades
  • Browse content in Environmental Science
  • Applied Ecology (Environmental Science)
  • Conservation of the Environment (Environmental Science)
  • Environmental Sustainability
  • Environmentalist Thought and Ideology (Environmental Science)
  • Management of Land and Natural Resources (Environmental Science)
  • Natural Disasters (Environmental Science)
  • Nuclear Issues (Environmental Science)
  • Pollution and Threats to the Environment (Environmental Science)
  • Social Impact of Environmental Issues (Environmental Science)
  • History of Science and Technology
  • Browse content in Materials Science
  • Ceramics and Glasses
  • Composite Materials
  • Metals, Alloying, and Corrosion
  • Nanotechnology
  • Browse content in Mathematics
  • Applied Mathematics
  • Biomathematics and Statistics
  • History of Mathematics
  • Mathematical Education
  • Mathematical Finance
  • Mathematical Analysis
  • Numerical and Computational Mathematics
  • Probability and Statistics
  • Pure Mathematics
  • Browse content in Neuroscience
  • Cognition and Behavioural Neuroscience
  • Development of the Nervous System
  • Disorders of the Nervous System
  • History of Neuroscience
  • Invertebrate Neurobiology
  • Molecular and Cellular Systems
  • Neuroendocrinology and Autonomic Nervous System
  • Neuroscientific Techniques
  • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • Browse content in Physics
  • Astronomy and Astrophysics
  • Atomic, Molecular, and Optical Physics
  • Biological and Medical Physics
  • Classical Mechanics
  • Computational Physics
  • Condensed Matter Physics
  • Electromagnetism, Optics, and Acoustics
  • History of Physics
  • Mathematical and Statistical Physics
  • Measurement Science
  • Nuclear Physics
  • Particles and Fields
  • Plasma Physics
  • Quantum Physics
  • Relativity and Gravitation
  • Semiconductor and Mesoscopic Physics
  • Browse content in Psychology
  • Affective Sciences
  • Clinical Psychology
  • Cognitive Psychology
  • Cognitive Neuroscience
  • Criminal and Forensic Psychology
  • Developmental Psychology
  • Educational Psychology
  • Evolutionary Psychology
  • Health Psychology
  • History and Systems in Psychology
  • Music Psychology
  • Neuropsychology
  • Organizational Psychology
  • Psychological Assessment and Testing
  • Psychology of Human-Technology Interaction
  • Psychology Professional Development and Training
  • Research Methods in Psychology
  • Social Psychology
  • Browse content in Social Sciences
  • Browse content in Anthropology
  • Anthropology of Religion
  • Human Evolution
  • Medical Anthropology
  • Physical Anthropology
  • Regional Anthropology
  • Social and Cultural Anthropology
  • Theory and Practice of Anthropology
  • Browse content in Business and Management
  • Business Ethics
  • Business Strategy
  • Business History
  • Business and Technology
  • Business and Government
  • Business and the Environment
  • Comparative Management
  • Corporate Governance
  • Corporate Social Responsibility
  • Entrepreneurship
  • Health Management
  • Human Resource Management
  • Industrial and Employment Relations
  • Industry Studies
  • Information and Communication Technologies
  • International Business
  • Knowledge Management
  • Management and Management Techniques
  • Operations Management
  • Organizational Theory and Behaviour
  • Pensions and Pension Management
  • Public and Nonprofit Management
  • Strategic Management
  • Supply Chain Management
  • Browse content in Criminology and Criminal Justice
  • Criminal Justice
  • Criminology
  • Forms of Crime
  • International and Comparative Criminology
  • Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice
  • Development Studies
  • Browse content in Economics
  • Agricultural, Environmental, and Natural Resource Economics
  • Asian Economics
  • Behavioural Finance
  • Behavioural Economics and Neuroeconomics
  • Econometrics and Mathematical Economics
  • Economic History
  • Economic Systems
  • Economic Methodology
  • Economic Development and Growth
  • Financial Markets
  • Financial Institutions and Services
  • General Economics and Teaching
  • Health, Education, and Welfare
  • History of Economic Thought
  • International Economics
  • Labour and Demographic Economics
  • Law and Economics
  • Macroeconomics and Monetary Economics
  • Microeconomics
  • Public Economics
  • Urban, Rural, and Regional Economics
  • Welfare Economics
  • Browse content in Education
  • Adult Education and Continuous Learning
  • Care and Counselling of Students
  • Early Childhood and Elementary Education
  • Educational Equipment and Technology
  • Educational Strategies and Policy
  • Higher and Further Education
  • Organization and Management of Education
  • Philosophy and Theory of Education
  • Schools Studies
  • Secondary Education
  • Teaching of a Specific Subject
  • Teaching of Specific Groups and Special Educational Needs
  • Teaching Skills and Techniques
  • Browse content in Environment
  • Applied Ecology (Social Science)
  • Climate Change
  • Conservation of the Environment (Social Science)
  • Environmentalist Thought and Ideology (Social Science)
  • Natural Disasters (Environment)
  • Social Impact of Environmental Issues (Social Science)
  • Browse content in Human Geography
  • Cultural Geography
  • Economic Geography
  • Political Geography
  • Browse content in Interdisciplinary Studies
  • Communication Studies
  • Museums, Libraries, and Information Sciences
  • Browse content in Politics
  • African Politics
  • Asian Politics
  • Chinese Politics
  • Comparative Politics
  • Conflict Politics
  • Elections and Electoral Studies
  • Environmental Politics
  • European Union
  • Foreign Policy
  • Gender and Politics
  • Human Rights and Politics
  • Indian Politics
  • International Relations
  • International Organization (Politics)
  • International Political Economy
  • Irish Politics
  • Latin American Politics
  • Middle Eastern Politics
  • Political Behaviour
  • Political Economy
  • Political Institutions
  • Political Methodology
  • Political Communication
  • Political Philosophy
  • Political Sociology
  • Political Theory
  • Politics and Law
  • Politics of Development
  • Public Policy
  • Public Administration
  • Quantitative Political Methodology
  • Regional Political Studies
  • Russian Politics
  • Security Studies
  • State and Local Government
  • UK Politics
  • US Politics
  • Browse content in Regional and Area Studies
  • African Studies
  • Asian Studies
  • East Asian Studies
  • Japanese Studies
  • Latin American Studies
  • Middle Eastern Studies
  • Native American Studies
  • Scottish Studies
  • Browse content in Research and Information
  • Research Methods
  • Browse content in Social Work
  • Addictions and Substance Misuse
  • Adoption and Fostering
  • Care of the Elderly
  • Child and Adolescent Social Work
  • Couple and Family Social Work
  • Developmental and Physical Disabilities Social Work
  • Direct Practice and Clinical Social Work
  • Emergency Services
  • Human Behaviour and the Social Environment
  • International and Global Issues in Social Work
  • Mental and Behavioural Health
  • Social Justice and Human Rights
  • Social Policy and Advocacy
  • Social Work and Crime and Justice
  • Social Work Macro Practice
  • Social Work Practice Settings
  • Social Work Research and Evidence-based Practice
  • Welfare and Benefit Systems
  • Browse content in Sociology
  • Childhood Studies
  • Community Development
  • Comparative and Historical Sociology
  • Economic Sociology
  • Gender and Sexuality
  • Gerontology and Ageing
  • Health, Illness, and Medicine
  • Marriage and the Family
  • Migration Studies
  • Occupations, Professions, and Work
  • Organizations
  • Population and Demography
  • Race and Ethnicity
  • Social Theory
  • Social Movements and Social Change
  • Social Research and Statistics
  • Social Stratification, Inequality, and Mobility
  • Sociology of Religion
  • Sociology of Education
  • Sport and Leisure
  • Urban and Rural Studies
  • Browse content in Warfare and Defence
  • Defence Strategy, Planning, and Research
  • Land Forces and Warfare
  • Military Administration
  • Military Life and Institutions
  • Naval Forces and Warfare
  • Other Warfare and Defence Issues
  • Peace Studies and Conflict Resolution
  • Weapons and Equipment

The Oxford Handbook of Applied Linguistics (2nd edn)

  • < Previous chapter
  • Next chapter >

The Oxford Handbook of Applied Linguistics (2nd edn)

15 Interactionist Perspectives on Second Language Acquisition

Susan Gass is University Distinguished Professor in the Department of Linguistics, Germanic, Slavic, Asian, and African Languages at Michigan State University. She is the director of the English Language Center, codirector of the Center for Language Education and Research, and director of the Second Language Studies PhD program. She has published widely in the field of SLA, focusing on a number of different areas, including language transfer, language universals, and input and interaction. She is the author of Input, Interaction, and the Second Language Learner, coauthor with Larry Selinker of Second Language Acquisition: An Introductory Course, and coauthor with Alison Mackey of Second Language Research: Methodology and Design and Data Elicitation for Second and Foreign Language Research. She can be reached at http://[email protected].

  • Published: 18 September 2012
  • Cite Icon Cite
  • Permissions Icon Permissions

This article analyses the idea of second language acquisition form an interactionist perspective. The field of second language acquisition has been studied from many angles. This broad scope is due in part to the myriad disciplinary backgrounds of scholars in the field. This article deals with the interactionist perspective and, as such, is primarily concerned with the environment in which second language learning takes place. It is important to note from the outset that this perspective is by and large neutral as to the role of innateness. In other words, it is compatible with a view of second language acquisition that posits an innate learning mechanism; it is also compatible with a model of learning that posits no such mechanism. This article deals with interactionist approaches focusing on how learners use their linguistic environment to build their knowledge of the second language. To summarize, the interaction approach considers, production of language as constructs that are important for understanding second language learning.

I. Introduction

The field of second language acquisition (SLA) has been studied from many angles. This broad scope is due in part to the myriad disciplinary backgrounds of scholars in the field. This chapter deals with the interactionist perspective and, as such, is primarily concerned with the environment in which second language learning takes place. It is important to note from the outset that this perspective is by and large neutral as to the role of innateness. In other words, it is compatible with a view of SLA that posits an innate learning mechanism (e.g., universal grammar [UG]); it is also compatible with a model of learning that posits no such mechanism. A word of caution is in order, however, as the situation is far more complex than these few terse statements suggest. It is not the case that interaction does not relate to issues of UG. Rather, what is intended is that the relationship of interaction to acquisition per se does not depend on whether there is or is not an innate mechanism that guides the learning of a second language. To provide an example of what the relationship between interaction and a UG account of learning might be, one notes that there are two kinds of evidence that are available to learners: 1 positive evidence and negative evidence. Positive evidence refers to the language that a learner hears or reads and is clearly available through the linguistic environment; negative evidence is more complex. Negative evidence refers to information about what is incorrect in the language produced by a learner but not necessarily about what is needed to make an appropriate correction. Thus, overt correction is a form of negative evidence that not only indicates an error but also provides information about what is wrong. It may be that for complex syntax, negative evidence is necessary; it may also be that negative evidence is difficult to provide in other than an explicit (pedagogical) format. In other words, issues that form part of learnability theory (e.g., UG) may find answers in the conversational interactions in which learners engage in that these interactions provide the forum for both positive and negative evidence.

I have argued elsewhere (Gass, 1997 ) that the dichotomy between innatist and environmental approaches is ill-conceived in the sense that a presumed dichotomy leads only to a discussion of “which is correct” rather than of “how they complement one another.” But the goal of second language research must be the determination of how these approaches are intertwined, assuming that both are indeed relevant, even though they provide different sorts of explanations (see also Pinker, 1994 ). This chapter deals with interactionist approaches focusing on how learners use their linguistic environment (in particular, conversational interactions) to build their knowledge of the second language. To summarize briefly, the interaction approach considers exposure to language (input), production of language (output), and feedback on production (through interaction) as constructs that are important for understanding how second language learning takes place. Gass 2003 argues that interaction research “takes as its starting point the assumption that language learning is stimulated by communicative pressure and examines the relationship between communication and acquisition and the mechanisms (e.g., noticing, attention) that mediate between them” (p. 224). Long ( 1996 ) makes a similar claim, proposing that “environmental contributions to acquisition are mediated by selective attention and the learner's developing L2 processing capacity,” and that these resources “are brought together most usefully … during negotiation for meaning . Negative feedback obtained … may be facilitative of L2 development” (p. 414). A related construct, output was emphasized in Swain's ( 1993 ) research: “Learners need to be pushed to make use of their resources; they need to have their linguistic abilities stretched to their fullest; they need to reflect on their output and consider ways of modifying it to enhance comprehensibility, appropriateness, and accuracy” (pp. 160–161).

In what follows, I discuss the basic tenets of the interaction approach (see Gass and Mackey, 2007a ) and then discuss learner-internal variables that impact how learners do and do not utilize language information stemming from interaction.

Input has had an uneven history in the development of (second) language research. In behaviorist views of language learning, input was central to an understanding of how learners acquired a language, first or second. Imitation and habit formation were primary concepts in the acquisition process. If habits were formed through imitation, then it was necessary to examine the input to learners to determine what they were imitating. It was also necessary to examine the relationship between the input (what was to be imitated) and production (the product of imitation).

From the mid-1950s on, behaviorism was on the wane, and as a consequence so too was the importance ascribed to input. A new era of language research emerged, and acquisition research followed, deemphasizing the significance of input and focusing on the nature of the internal linguistic resources that a learner brings to the learning task.

By the early 1970s, scholars began to take a more balanced view of what was relevant and not relevant to the study of second language learning. With specific regard to input, Ferguson 1971 , 1975 ) began to investigate the nature of input to nonproficient speakers of a language. In particular, he considered special registers such as “baby talk” (i.e., language addressed to young children) and “foreigner talk” (i.e., language addressed to nonnative speakers of a language). His focus was on the similarities between these two language systems, and his ultimate goal was understanding the nature of human language. With regard to foreigner talk, the system of particular interest to researchers in second language acquisition, certain common features became apparent. Speech directed towards nonproficient nonnative speakers was found to include speech modification ranging from phonological to syntactic. For example, speech tends to be slower, more clearly enunciated, and even louder. In terms of the lexicon, vocabulary tends to have a preponderance of common words. Syntax is simple, often including two sentences when one might normally expect to find one complex sentence. These characteristics are commonly found in speech to learners, although clearly there is variation among individuals. 2

What function does modified speech serve? A lengthier discussion and exposé on this topic may be found in Gass 1997 . For present purposes, it is important to point out that there are two perspectives from which one can answer this question. First is the perspective of the fluent speaker of the target language. It is likely that the purpose is to aid comprehension. One does what one can to ensure that one's conversational partner is able minimally to understand the general meaning and to be in a position to respond appropriately. The example below shows how a native speaker (NS), upon realizing that an original question may have been too difficult for a learner (NNS), modifies her speech to give the learner a greater opportunity to comprehend.

NNS : How have increasing food costs changed your eating habits? ➔NS : Uh well that would I don't think they've changed 'em much right now, but the pressure's on. NNS : Pardon me? ➔NS : I don't think they've changed our eating habits much as of now…. (From Gass and Varonis, 1985 )

In this example the reduced pronoun ('em ➔ them) is modified to include full information (our eating habits).

A second way of considering the function of modified speech is to examine the question from the perspective of the learner. Modified speech contributes to the likelihood that the learner can understand and can therefore get through what is essentially a social interaction. In other words, modified speech helps the learner participate in a conversation as fully as possible. Assuming that a learner is able to participate in a conversation, she is assured of receiving a greater quantity of input, can produce output, and can receive much-needed feedback on his/her production.

III. Interaction

The interactionist hypothesis—which was given initial prominence by Wagner-Gough and Hatch 1975 and refined by Long ( 1980 , 1981 , 1983 ) and others (Gass and Varonis, 1985 , 1989 ; Mackey, 1999 ; Pica, 1987 , 1988 ; Pica and Doughty, 1985 ; Pica, Doughty, and Young, 1986 ; Pica, Young, and Doughty, 1987 ; Schmidt and Frota, 1986 ; Varonis and Gass, 1985a )—has as its main claim that one route to second language learning is through conversational interaction (Gass, 1997 ; Long, 1996 ; Pica, 1994 ).

In Wagner-Gough and Hatch's original work, the role of conversation was valued, not just as a means for providing opportunities to practice previously learned language, but also as a locus of learning itself. Long, in his early work, showed how conversations involving nonfluent nonnative speakers of a language were quantitatively different from conversations in which both parties (assuming dyadic conversation) were equal and fluent participants. Wagner-Gough and Hatch 1975 and Long ( 1980 , 1981 , 1983 ) went beyond modified speech (e.g., simpler syntactic structures, easier vocabulary) to consider the structure of conversation itself. Following are examples of typical patterns found in conversations involving nonfluent, nonnative speakers of a language. This is not to say that these patterns do not exist in conversations involving fluent speakers—only that they are more frequent in nonnative learner speech.

Confirmation Check

NNS : c ' è una verdi, uh … there is a green, uh. NS : una verdi? a green? (From Mackey, Gass, and McDonough, 2000 )

In this example from an English-speaking learner of Italian, the NS's questioning the word verdi , which in fact is inappropriate in Italian, resulted in a subsequent negotiation until the correct word pianta (plant) was recognized later in the exchange.

Examples 3–7 are crucial to an understanding of the interaction hypothesis and to an understanding of how modified interactions contribute to learning. In these examples, one can see various means of modifying a conversational structure, resulting in a greater likelihood of comprehension.

Comprehension Check

NNS1 : And your family have some ingress. NNS2 : yes ah, OK, OK? ➔ NNS1 : more or less OK? (From Varonis and Gass, 1985a )

Clarification request

NS : there's there's just a couple more things NNS : a sorry? Couple? NS : couple more things in the room only just a couple ➔ NNS : couple? What does it mean couple? (From Mackey and Philp, 1998 )
NS : Where did you go yesterday? NNS : What? ➔NS : Did you go to the zoo or to the garden? (Original data)

Topic-focused

NS : Did your friend travel with you to Italy and Switzerland? NNS : What? ➔ NS : Your friend, did she travel with you? (Original data)

Elaboration

NNS : How have increasing food costs changed your eating habits? NS : Well, we don't eat as much beef as we used to. We eat more chicken and uh, pork, and uh, fish, things like that. NNS : Pardon me? ➔ NS : We don't eat as much beef as we used to. We eat more chicken and uh, uh pork and fish…. We don't eat beef very often. We don't have steak like we used to. (From Gass and Varonis, 1985 )

Long ( 1996 ) defined the interaction hypothesis as

negotiation for meaning , and especially negotiation work that triggers interactional adjustments by the NS or more competent interlocutor, facilitates acquisition because it connects input, internal learner capacities, particularly selective attention, and output in productive ways. (pp. 451–452)

In addition,

it is proposed that environmental contributions to acquisition are mediated by selective attention and the learner's developing L2 processing capacity, and that these resources are brought together most usefully, although not exclusively, during negotiation for meaning . Negative feedback obtained during negotiation work or elsewhere may be facilitative of L2 development, at least for vocabulary, morphology, and language-specific syntax, and essential for learning certain specifiable L1-L2 contrasts. (p. 414)

It is through negotiation that the learner may direct attention to an area of the target language (1) about which she may be entertaining an hypothesis (or about which she is trying to formulate a hypothesis) or (2) about which she has no information. This is not to say that learning necessarily takes place during a conversation; the interaction itself may only be the first step in recognizing that there is something to learn. Interaction may be a priming device representing the setting of the stage for learning. The following examples represent two ways in which conversation can lead to learning: (1) on the spot learning and (2) delayed learning.

Example 8 comes from a study that used stimulated recall (see Gass and Mackey, 2000 ) to determine what a learner was thinking during a prior interaction. The relevant part of the interaction is provided, followed immediately by the learner's comments on the interaction.

On the Spot Learning

NNS : so the people make a line in front of the place NS : they are standing in line? ➔ NNS : ahh, they are standing in line (Twelve turns later) ➔ NNS : Beside the people standing in line (From Mackey, Gass, and McDonough, 2000 )
Learner's retrospective comment: Make a line is the same meaning as stand in line, actually I thought of this situation as make a line but after she said standing in line, her expression is better than mine so I changed mine.

The learner in this instance used the conversation as a way of obtaining new linguistic information, as is evidenced by her retrospective comments. She then tried out her newly learned information later in the conversation.

But sometimes learning does not take place immediately; time to “digest” the new linguistic input is often needed. This is illustrated in 9, which took place over the time period of a class.

Delayed Learning

➔NNS1 : Kutsu o shiroi no kutsu o hamete imasu ka? Shoes ACC white (GEN [ sic ] shoes ACC wearing [ sic ] INT? Is the person wearing white shoes” ((wrong verb for “wear,” underlined)) NNS2 : Iie haite imasen . Ano::: No, he's not. Uh:: (correct verb for “wear,” underlined)) ➔NNS1 : Ano: ano: kuroi no kutsu o hai- hamete imasu ka? U:h u:h black (GEN [ sic ] shoes ACC wea-wearing [ sic ] INT? U'h u”h is the person wearing black shoes ((wrong word for “wear,” underlined)) NNS2 : Hai haite imasu Yes, he is ((correct verb for “wear,” underlined)) NNS1 : Anoo uh jiinzu o ::: jeans? Jinzu o ha uh :::: haite imasu ka? U:h uh: you're um jeans? Are you u :::h wearing Jeans? NNS1 : Sorekara shiroi no kutsu ga kutsu o how do you say it? Kutso o ::: And white shoes shoes how do you say it for shoes? NNS3 : haitemasu. haki-Wearing. Wear- ➔NNS1 : what? Haite imasu ka? What? Is it “haite imasu?” ➔NNS4 : Hanjiinzu o : : um oh what is it? Hare- haite imasu ka? Shorts (( sic )) ACC um oh what is it? Do you we-wear shorts? NNS4 : (inaudible) ➔NNS1 : Sorekara ::: kiroi to shiroi no kutsu g- o ::: yeah. Haite imasu ka? And ::: are you wearing yellow and white shoes? (From Ohta, 2001 )

In this example the learner initially uses the incorrect form for the verb wear, haimete , rather than the correct form, haite . She begins to receive input with the correct form and eventually asks about the correct form, but it isn't until quite a bit later in the class that she finally, presumably as a result of the interaction, begins to use the correct form.

Learners do not always use a conversation to obtain new information; there are times when she or he uses it to test certain hypotheses about the second language and to receive feedback on production. Example 10 below shows how a learner uses a conversation to test a hypothesis:

NNS : poi un bicchiere then a glass INT : un che, come a what, what? NNS : bicchiere Glass (From research reported on in Mackey, Gass, and McDonough, 2000 )
Learner's retrospective comment: I was drawing a blank. Then I thought of a vase but then I thought that since there was no flowers, maybe it was just a big glass. So, then I thought I'll say it and see. Then, when she said “ come ” [what] I knew that it was completely wrong.

In this instance, the learner is throwing out a word to see where it gets her.

3.1 Recasts

Feedback to learners can be explicit (e.g., an overt correction) or implicit, as often illustrated through negotiation work. Perhaps the most subtle type of feedback comes in the form of a recast, a reformulation of an incorrect utterance while maintaining the original meaning of the utterance. An example is given in 11 in which the native speaker recasts the nonnative speaker's incorrect question:

NNS : I think some this girl have birthday and and its big celebrate ➔NS : big celebration NNS : oh (From Mackey and Philip, 1998 )

A question arises as to the effectiveness of recasts. Are they noticed, and, if so, are they taken as a form of correction? Lyster and Ranta 1997 collected data from grades 4–6 children in French immersion programs. They were primarily concerned with the reaction by the student immediately following a recast inasmuch as this reveals what the student does with the feedback. Despite the preponderance of recasts in their database, recasts were not found to have an impact on subsequent production. Using the same database, Lyster 1998 found that there was some confusion between the corrective and approval functions of recasts, thereby questioning their usefulness in terms of corrective feedback.

Other studies, however, do show a positive effect for recasts (for reviews, see Long, 2007 , and Nicholas, Lightbown, and Spada, 2001 ). Mackey and Philp 1998 argue that using the production immediately following a recast may not be the most appropriate way to determine effectiveness. They make the point that if one is to consider effectiveness (i.e., development/acquisition), then one should more appropriately measure delayed effects (see, e.g., Gass, 1997 ; Gass and Varonis, 1994 ; Lightbown, 1998 ). The study on the acquisition of English questions (Mackey and Philp, 1998 ) showed that for more advanced learners, recasts plus negotiation were more beneficial than negotiation alone. This was the case even though there was not always evidence for a reaction by the learner in the subsequent turn (see Oliver, 1995 ). Thus, the effectiveness of recasts may be in part dependent on developmental stages (see, e.g., Iwashita, 2003 ; Philp, 2003 ). Further, as Egi ( 2007 ) has shown, the value of recasts may be dependent on their interpretation—in other words, are they thought to be a comment on content (a practice that is not effective), or are they taken to be a form of positive and/or negative evidence (in which case they appear to be of some benefit)?

Recent meta-analyses (Mackey and Goo, 2007 ; Russell and Spada, 2006 ) have addressed the effects of recasts. Both studies reported large effect sizes for implicit feedback (including recasts), but both also report that their data are based on very few studies given their inclusion criteria for their meta-analyses. Mackey and Goo also note that although there are strong effects on learning from recasts, there is little data on longer term effects.

3.2 Does Interaction Contribute to Learning?

In recent years, the question has moved from the arena of speculation to serious investigation of the effects of interaction. Three early studies led the way in this regard:

Loschky 1994 , in an investigation of English learners of Japanese, found that interaction had a positive effect on comprehension but did not find an effect on the acquisition of vocabulary or on the acquisition of morphosyntax (locative expressions; see also Ellis, Tanaka, and Yamazaki ( 1994 ).

Gass and Varonis 1994 did show that the effects of interaction went beyond a subsequent turn. As in previous studies, vocabulary did not appear to be affected, although general discourse organizational strategies were affected.

Mackey 1999 provides the most detailed support in favor of the effects of interaction. In her investigation of a single grammatical structure, question formation, she found a relationship between conversational interaction and development in that those who were involved in structure-focused interaction moved along a developmental continuum more rapidly than those who did not. Mackey's study supports the notion of interaction not necessarily (or not always) being the locus of immediate learning but often being the catalyst for later learning. She found that for the developmentally advanced structures, the effects of interaction were noted in delayed posttests rather than immediately.

In the past 30 years or so, numerous studies have been conducted on the relationship between interaction and learning, most showing a positive relationship (e.g., Adams, 2007 ; Trofimovich, Ammar and Gatbonton, 2007 ; Carpenter, Jeon, MacGregor, and Mackey, 2006 ; Ellis, 2007 ; Ellis, Loewen, and Erlam, 2006 ; Gass and Alvarez-Torres, 2005 ; Gass and Lewis, 2007 ; Jeon, 2007 ; Kim and Han, 2007 ; Loewen, 2005 ; Loewen and Nabei, 2007 ; Loewen and Philp, 2006 ; Lyster and Mori, 2006 ; Mackey, 2006 ; Mackey and Silver, 2005 ; McDonough, 2005 , 2006 , 2007 ; McDonough and Mackey, 2006 ; Pica, Kang, and Sauro, 2006 ; Sachs and Suh, 2007 ; Sagarra, 2007 ; Sato and Lyster, 2007 ; Sheen, 2007 ; Tarone and Bigelow, 2007 ; Tocalli-Beller and Swain, 2007 ). Only studies published since 2005 have been included due to limitations of space; for a more complete listing, see Mackey ( 2007 ).

In their meta-analysis, Mackey and Goo ( 2007 ) found that interaction is facilitative of the acquisition of both vocabulary and grammar. There is a stronger immediate effect for vocabulary, but a delayed and lasting effect on grammar. Both feedback and modified output were significant factors in promoting learning, but Mackey and Goo recommend a more nuanced investigation of these interactional components.

There is little doubt as to the facilitative effects of interaction on learning, but many questions remain unanswered. Some of the questions are descriptive, as was previously alluded to; that is, what types of feedback contribute to learning and under what circumstances? The second type of question is explanatory; that is, why does interaction contribute to learning? There are four areas that will be examined next:

Working memory (including inhibitory control)

IV. Attention

Central to the interaction hypothesis is the concept of attention or noticing. If interaction is to have an effect (either through negotiation or recasts), the learner must notice that his/her conversational partner is explicitly or implicitly making a correction. 3 If there is no attention to a particular part of language during an interaction, then it is difficult to attribute the source of change to the interaction itself. Example 12 illustrates how the direct questioning of an utterance makes the learner notice the discrepancy between her pronunciation of yellow and the native speaker interviewer's pronunciation:

NNS : The color is / wellow / NS : Is what? NNS : /wellow/ /wellow/ color NS : Yellow? NNS : Yellow. (From Mackey, Gass and McDonough, 2000 )
Learner's retrospective comment: My pronounce is different. I say /wellow/ but yellow is the exact pronounce. Yellow, yellow.

The question remains as to what learners do notice. J. Williams 1999 considered learner-generated attention to form. She points out that most of the focus on form research within a classroom context focuses on teacher-generated attention. (See also Sharwood Smith's [ 1991 , 1993 ] discussion of enhanced input and internally and externally induced salience). Williams found that learners are indeed capable of focusing attention to language form, but that there is variation according to proficiency level and even to activity types.

Mackey, Gass, and McDonough ( 2000 ) also investigated what learners notice in an interaction. Their investigation differed from that of Williams in that they were concerned with interactional feedback and how learners actually interpreted that feedback. Through a postinteraction stimulated recall of learners of English and learners of Italian, they found that learners do notice interactional feedback, but they do not do so in a uniform manner. Lexis and phonology are more likely to be noticed than aspects of morphosyntax. The results also suggest that the manner of feedback (e.g., recasts or negotiation) have different effects when occurring alone or in combination. A related study of heritage and nonheritage learners (Gass and Lewis, 2007 ) found that both nonheritage language learners and heritage language learners perceive phonological and lexical feedback much more accurately than morphosyntactic feedback, as had been observed in Mackey, Gass, and McDonough ( 2000 ). However, differences were noted in the area of semantic feedback, with nonheritage language learners generally not accurate in their perceptions about semantic feedback, whereas heritage language learners were. Neither of these studies investigated the next step; that is, the determination of what happens after learners notice a gap between their knowledge of the second language and the second language itself.

V. Working Memory

Within an interactive context, learners produce language, receive feedback, and, in an ideal situation, use the information contained in the feedback in a productive way. In so doing, a learner must notice the feedback given, determine what is relevant, and retain that information long enough to identify the precise part of language that is being corrected. The question remains as to why some individuals are more successful at this than others. One possible explanation relates to working memory capacity. Why some learners are able to focus attention on certain parts of an interaction better than others may be related to their ability to regulate their focus of attention, either through selectively attending to some part of language or by inhibiting others. Working memory is generally considered to incorporate both processing and storage functions of memory. Miyake and Shah 1999 define working memory as “those mechanisms or processes that are involved in the control, regulation, and active maintenance of task-relevant information in the service of complex cognition, including novel as well as familiar, skilled tasks” (p. 450). In other words, following work by Baddeley and Hitch ( 1974 ), working memory keeps representations in temporary storage, allowing operations on those representations to take place (Caplan, Waters, and Dede, 2007 ).

There have not been many studies that relate interactional success and working memory capacity. One of the early ones was by Philp 2003 , who studied noticing. She did not measure working memory capacity, but did point the way to research involving working memory when she suggested that attentional resources may be at the base of understanding when learners do notice differences between their own utterances and those of the target language. Two studies by Sagarra ( 2007 , 2008 ) indirectly looked at working memory capacity and interaction. In one—a computer-based study of feedback—Sagarra ( 2007 ) found that working memory capacity predicted learners' ability to benefit from recasts. In the second, Sagarra ( 2008 ) found that redundant grammatical information is not processed by low proficiency L2 learners (English as an L1; Spanish as an L2) with low working memory capacity.

In the most directly relevant study, Mackey, Philp, Egi, Fujii, and Tatsumi ( 2002 ) investigated the relationship among individual differences in verbal working memory, noticing of interactional feedback, and the L2 development of English question formation. Their study compared the interactional benefits of two groups of L2 learners (L1 Japanese; L2 English): those with low working memory capacity and those with high working memory capacity. The former group benefitted immediately from interaction, but the results did not persist on a delayed posttest (2 weeks after the treatment). Those with higher working memory capacity demonstrated more lasting benefits from communicative interaction as demonstrated in the delayed posttest. A possible explanation is that learners with higher working memory capacities engage in cognitive comparisons between target language forms and their own versions of the forms, impacting processing loads and immediate performance. Learners with lower working memory capacities, in contrast, may be better equipped to engage in immediate modifications to output, at a potential longer term cost to comparison, storage, and subsequent retrieval mechanisms. Thus, the emphasis for high-working memory capacity individuals may be on processing, whereas the emphasis for low-working memory capacity individuals may be on storage (for differing results, cf. Trofimovich, Ammar, and Gatbonton, 2007 ).

Initial analysis in a study of English-speaking learners of Italian (Gass, Behney, and Uzum, in preparation) suggests that more significant than working memory differences are differences in inhibitory control (a construct related to working memory)—that is, the ability to suppress information that is not relevant.

In sum, second language learners are exposed to more input than they can process; consequently, they must have a mechanism that enables them to sort through that input (see, e.g., Gass, 1997 ) to determine what is (momentarily) relevant and what is not. Working memory capacity may be one such mechanism and inhibitory control may be another.

In preceding sections, the nature of language that is directed toward learners and the function of modified language and/or modified conversational structure have been considered. However, in any discussion of the interaction hypothesis, there is a third prong to examine, and that is the role of output. In earlier conceptualizations of second language acquisition, output served little learning purpose, other than, perhaps, to reinforce previously learned linguistic knowledge. Swain's ( 1985 ) pioneering work (see also Swain, 1995 , 2005 ) in this area came from observations of immersion programs in Canada (see also Kowal and Swain, 1997 ; Swain and Lapkin, 1995 , 1998 ). She noted that children who had spent many years in immersion programs were still lagging in target-language-like abilities. In looking more carefully at the classroom context in which the target language was used and in which it was the prime source of information about the target language for these children, she noted that what was lacking was consistent and frequent use of the second language. She proposed that one needed more than input; learning a second language required a significant amount of output. Output, or language production, forces learners to focus on the syntax of an utterance and, consequently, on formulating hypotheses about how the target language works. This is different from receiving input because input involves primarily comprehension and comprehension often requires little syntactic organization. As a result, Swain introduced the notion of comprehensible output or “pushed” output.

Comprehensible output refers to the need for a learner to be “pushed toward the delivery of a message that is not only conveyed, but that is conveyed precisely, coherently, and appropriately” (Swain, 1985 : 249). In a more recent explication of the concept, Swain claimed output may stimulate learners to move from the semantic, open-ended, nondeterministic, strategic processing prevalent in comprehension to the complete grammatical processing needed for accurate production. Output, thus, would seem to have a potentially significant role in the development of syntax and morphology (1995: 128).

Mackey 2002 conducted a study in which learners reflected on a previous interaction through a stimulated recall procedure. Example 13 provides insight into what the learner was thinking as she was engaged in a conversation.

Example of Pushed Output

NNS: And in hand in hand [ sic ] have a bigger glass to see. NS: It's err. You mean, something in his hand? NNS: Like spectacle. For older person. NS: Mmmm, sorry I don't follow, it's what? NNS: In hand have he have has a glass for looking through for make the print bigger to see, to see the print, for magnify. NS: He has some glasses? NNS: Magnify glasses he has magnifying glass. NS: Oh aha I see a magnifying glass, right that's a good one, ok. (Mackey 2002 )
Retrospective comment: In this example I see I have to manage myerrerr [ sic ] expression because he does not understand me and I cannot think of exact word right then. I am thinking thinking [ sic ] it is nearly in my mind, thinking bigger and magnificate and eventually magnify. I know I see this word before but so I am sort of talking around around [ sic ] this word but he is forcing me to think harder, think harder for the correct word to give him so he can understand and so I was trying. I carry on talking until finally I get it, and when I say it, then he understand it, me [emphasis mine].

As can be seen, the learner was pushed (note the word forcing ) through the negotiation sequences to make her language clearer. 4

McDonough 2005 tested the output hypothesis directly in her study of Thai learners of English. In a study investigating the acquisition of English questions, four groups carried out communicative tasks. The groups focused on salience (enhancement) and opportunity to modify following feedback. She found that the best predictor of acquisition lies in the opportunity to modify one's speech. A later study (McDonough and Mackey, 2006 ), however, noted that mere repetition of an interlocutor's form (in their case, recasts) did not impact learning, whereas primed production (use of a form in the recast in later production) did.

Output, in sum, is important for a number of reasons, including forcing a learner to produce language about which feedback may often be given. However, precisely how it benefits learning is still open for investigation. Further, as noted by Sato and Lyster 2007 , the interlocutor partner (native speaker or another learner) may impact the amount of modified output; learners tend to produce more when interacting with other learners than with a native speaker (see also Varonis and Gass, 1985a ).

VII. Theory of Contrast

What sort of mechanism allows for learning to take place as a result of negative evidence derived from conversational interaction? One possibility to account for learning through conversation is the direct contrast hypothesis (Saxton, 1997 ), defined within the context of child language acquisition as follows:

When the child produces an utterance containing an erroneous form, which is responded to immediately with an utterance containing the correct adult alternative to the erroneous form (i.e., when negative evidence is supplied), the child may perceive the adult form as being in CONTRAST [emphasis in original] with the equivalent child form. Cognizance of a relevant contrast can then form the basis for perceiving the adult form as a correct alternative to the child form. (1997: 155)

Attention alone is not sufficient. A contrast must be attended to, or in SLA parlance, a gap must be noticed. And conversation provides a forum for the contrast to be detected, especially when the erroneous form and a correct one are in immediate juxtaposition.

VIII. Conclusion

It is likely that there are limitations to what can and cannot be learned through negative evidence provided through conversation. One possibility is that surface-level phenomena can be learned but abstractions cannot. This is consistent with Truscott's 1998 claim that competence is not affected by noticing. Negative evidence can probably not apply to long stretches of speech, given memory limitations (see Philp, 2003 ). But it may be effective with low-level phenomena such as pronunciation or the basic meanings of lexical items. In fact, these are precisely the areas that Mackey, Gass, and McDonough ( 2000 ) have isolated as those that are sensitive to feedback. Future research will need to determine the long-term effects of interaction on different parts of language (see also Gass, Svetics, and Lemelin, 2003 ; Trofimovic, Ammar, and Gatbonton, 2007 ).

Research on interaction began descriptively. In the past decade, as it became clear through empirical research that there was a positive relationship between interaction and learning, research moved toward an explanation of how this relationship works: What individual factors can help to explain differential benefits? In looking toward future research, individual cognitive differences will undoubtedly lead the way in attempting to answer this question, but other factors such as motivation, learning strategies, language aptitude, cognitive styles, learning context (e.g., second versus foreign language learning), language background (heritage versus nonheritage language learners), and social context will also likely have a role.

There is a third type of evidence, indirect negative evidence (see Plough, 1994 , 1995 ). To simplify matters, we do not deal with this complex type of evidence.

It is beyond the scope of this paper to comment on whether these systems are learned or not. It is important to note, however, that the extent to which an individual adopts foreigner talk characteristics in her speech may depend to an individual's experience with nonnative speakers. Varonis and Gass (1985b) describe an interaction in which a salesperson adopts very few foreigner talk features (she speaks rapidly, uses idioms, and uses anaphoric pronouns with no obvious referent). It was speculated that this is so precisely because of her lack of experience with noncomprehending individuals.

This is not to say that attention cannot come from a learner noticing a new form on her own (i.e., without an interlocutor's correction).

It is not clear from the description in the original article whether the learner had actually seen the phrase magnifying glass as part of the input (although she does say that she had seen the word [ sic ] before) and was trying to recall it or if she was generating it from what she had heard in the exchange. Regardless, it is through the interaction that this learner was able to come up with the correct word.

  • About Oxford Academic
  • Publish journals with us
  • University press partners
  • What we publish
  • New features  
  • Open access
  • Institutional account management
  • Rights and permissions
  • Get help with access
  • Accessibility
  • Advertising
  • Media enquiries
  • Oxford University Press
  • Oxford Languages
  • University of Oxford

Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University's objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide

  • Copyright © 2024 Oxford University Press
  • Cookie settings
  • Cookie policy
  • Privacy policy
  • Legal notice

This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an annual subscription.

  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Therapy Center
  • When To See a Therapist
  • Types of Therapy
  • Best Online Therapy
  • Best Couples Therapy
  • Best Family Therapy
  • Managing Stress
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Understanding Emotions
  • Self-Improvement
  • Healthy Relationships
  • Student Resources
  • Personality Types
  • Guided Meditations
  • Verywell Mind Insights
  • 2024 Verywell Mind 25
  • Mental Health in the Classroom
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Review Board
  • Crisis Support

What Is Sociocultural Theory?

Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

interaction hypothesis and sociocultural theory

Amy Morin, LCSW, is a psychotherapist and international bestselling author. Her books, including "13 Things Mentally Strong People Don't Do," have been translated into more than 40 languages. Her TEDx talk,  "The Secret of Becoming Mentally Strong," is one of the most viewed talks of all time.

interaction hypothesis and sociocultural theory

  • Zone of Proximal Development
  • Vygotsky vs. Piaget
  • Applications

Frequently Asked Questions

Sociocultural theory is an emerging field of psychology that looks at the contributions of society to individual development. This theory has become increasingly prominent since the 1990s and can be applied in educational settings as well as in socialization and play.

Psychologist Lev Vygotsky believed that parents, caregivers, peers, and the culture at large are responsible for developing the brain's higher-order functions . According to Vygotsky, human development relies on social interaction and, therefore, can differ among cultures.

Sociocultural theory stresses the role that social interaction plays in psychological development . It suggests that human learning is largely a social process, and that our cognitive functions are formed based on our interactions with those around us who are "more skilled."

According to the sociocultural perspective, our psychological growth is guided, in part, by people in our lives who are in mentor-type roles, such as teachers and parents. Other times, we develop our values and beliefs through our interactions within social groups or by participating in cultural events.

Sociocultural theory focuses on how mentors and peers influence individual learning, but also on how cultural beliefs and attitudes affect how learning takes place.

History of Sociocultural Theory

Sociocultural theory grew from the work of psychologist Lev Vygotsky , who believed that parents, caregivers, peers, and the culture at large are responsible for developing higher-order functions. According to Vygotsky, learning has its basis in interacting with other people. Once this has occurred, the information is then integrated on the individual level.

Vygotsky contended that children are born with basic biological constraints on their minds. Each culture, however, provides "tools of intellectual adaptation." These tools allow children to use their abilities in a way that is adaptive to the culture in which they live.

For example, one culture might emphasize memory strategies such as note-taking. Another might use tools like reminders or rote memorization (a technique that uses repetition). These nuances influence how a child learns, providing the "tools" that are appropriate to their culture.

Vygotsky, born in 1896, was a contemporary of other great thinkers such as Freud , Skinner , and Piaget , but his early death at age 37 and the suppression of his work in Stalinist Russia initially left his theories less well-known. As his work has become more widely published, his ideas have grown increasingly influential in areas including child development, cognitive psychology , and education.

The Zone of Proximal Development

An important concept in sociocultural theory is known as the zone of proximal development. According to Vygotsky, this is "the distance between the actual development level (of the learner) as determined by independent problem solving and the level of potential development as determined through problem solving under adult guidance, or in collaboration with more capable peers."

Essentially, it includes all of the knowledge and skills that a person cannot yet understand or perform on their own but is capable of learning with guidance. As children are allowed to stretch their skills and knowledge, often by observing someone who is slightly more advanced than they are, they are able to progressively extend this zone.

Some research has supported the validity of the zone of proximal development. For instance, one study reported that whether a student experiences test anxiety is influenced, in part, by whether they have someone available to provide assistance if needed. A 2013 case study connects this concept with how a student develops writing abilities.

Vygotsky vs. Piaget: Key Differences

Jean Piaget was a psychologist and genetic epistemologist known for his theory of cognitive development which outlines the four stages in which children learn. Since they are both theories of learning, Vygotsky's theory is often compared to Piaget's.

Social factors influence development

Development can differ between cultures

Childhood interactions and explorations influence development

Development is largely universal

How does Vygotsky's sociocultural theory differ from Piaget's theory of cognitive development ? First, while Piaget's theory stressed that a child's interactions and explorations impact development, Vygotsky asserted the essential role that social interactions play.

Another important difference between the two is that Piaget's theory suggests that development is largely universal and Vygotsky asserts that it can differ between cultures. The course of development in European culture, for example, might be different than in Asian culture.

Because cultures can vary so dramatically, Vygotsky's sociocultural theory suggests that both the course and content of intellectual development are not as universal as Piaget believed.

Some suggest that these two theories of human development differ greatly due to their founders' different upbringings and that Vygotsky had strong cultural ties while Piaget had a lonely childhood.

Applying Vygotsky's Theory

Sociocultural theory has gained popularity within certain settings. Here's how this theory can be put into practice in the real world.

In the Classroom

Understanding the zone of proximal development can be helpful for teachers. In classroom settings, teachers may first assess students to determine their current skill level. Educators can then offer instruction that stretches the limits of each child's capabilities.

At first, the student may need assistance from an adult or a more knowledgeable peer. Eventually, their zone of proximal development will expand. Teachers can help promote this expansion by:

  • Planning and organizing classroom instruction and lessons. For example, the teacher might organize the class into groups where less-skilled children are paired with students who have a higher skill level. 
  • Using hints, prompts, and direct instruction to help kids improve their ability levels.
  • Scaffolding , where the teacher provides specific prompts to move the child progressively forward toward a goal.

In Socialization and Play

Vygotsky's theory also stressed the importance of play in learning. Vygotsky believed that through playing and imagining, children are able to further stretch their conceptual abilities and knowledge of the world. 

Teachers and parents can use this concept by providing children with plenty of opportunities for play experiences . Types of play that can foster learning include imaginary play, role-playing, games, and reenactments of real events. Such activities help promote the growth of abstract thought.

A Word From Verywell

Although Vygotsky's sociocultural theory only gained credence after his death, research has helped validate the role that those around us play in shaping how we develop as individuals.

Even though not everyone agrees as to the specifics of this development, as outlined in Piaget vs. Vygotsky, the sociocultural perspective does contribute to this understanding. It has also influenced other modern theories of human development, such as those that relate to cognitive growth and education.

Creating a collaborative learning environment is one way to use sociocultural theory in the classroom. This might involve pairing students with others of higher skill levels, or it could be by learning as a group versus having students learn on their own.

Teachers can also take advantage of the zone of proximal development by providing guidance and support to help the students reach their learning goals—particularly in an online learning environment.

The sociocultural perspective reinforces the role that people in mentor-like positions play in shaping who we become. This includes not just parents and teachers but also community leaders and others we model ourselves after.

If you are in one of these positions, it's important to recognize that you are shaping the development of the children around you. Because sociocultural theory also stresses the importance that culture plays in the process, this can help us better understand how our traditions and customs can influence future generations.

Sociocultural theory explains learning as a social practice while cognitive theory considers learning on a more individual level. With cognitive theory, learning is dependent on a person's mental processes. Thus, it is more focused on how the human mind works versus the impact that society plays in development.

Stanish N. Evolutionary vs. sociocultural perspectives on human mate selection: The role of women's academic achievement on their need for financial stability . Eastern Illinois University.

American Psychological Association. Sociocultural perspective .

American Psychological Association. Tools of intellectual adaptation .

Britannica. L. S. Vygotsky: Soviet psychologist .

NYSED Office of Bilingual Education and World Language. The zone of proximal development: An affirmative perspective in teaching ELLs/MLLs .

Meijer J, Elshout JJ. The predictive and discriminant validity of the zone of proximal development . Br J Educ Psychol. 2001;71(Pt 1):93-113.

Thompson I. The mediation of learning in the zone of proximal development through a co-constructed writing activity . Res Teach Eng . 2013;47(3):247-76.

Forman EA, Kraker MJ. The social origins of logic: The contributions of Piaget and Vygotsky . New Dir Child Dev. 1985;(29):23-39.

Shaffer DR. Social and Personality Development . Thomson Wadsworth.

Quinn H. Jean Piaget and Lev Vygotsky . IU South Bend Undergraduate Res J . 2019;19.

Wass R, Golding C. Sharpening a tool for teaching: the zone of proximal development . Teaching Higher Ed . 2014;19(6):671-84. doi:10.1080/13562517.2014.901958

Van Oers B, Duijkers D. Teaching in a play-based curriculum: Theory, practice and evidence of developmental education for young children . J Curric Stud. 2013;45(4):511-34. doi:10.1080/00220272.2011.637182

Play and the learning environment . In: Gordon Biddle KA, Garcia-Nevarez A, Roundtree Henderson WJ, Valero-Kerrick A. Early Childhood Education: Becoming a Professional. Sage Publishing.

Schilhab T, Esbensen GL. Socio-cultural influences on situated cognition in nature . Front Psychol . 2019;10:980. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00980

Swain M, Kinnear P, Steinman L. Sociocultural theory in second language education: An introduction through narratives . Multilingual Matters . 2015. doi:10.21832/9781783093182

Jumaat N, Tasir Z. Instructional scaffolding in online learning environment: a meta-analysis . 2014 International Conference on Teaching and Learning in Computing and Engineering.

By Kendra Cherry, MSEd Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

VYGOTSKY ¬ð SSOCIOCULTURAL THEORY AND THE ROLE OF INPUT AND OUTPUTIN SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION

Profile image of Khaerudin 0406

¡¡¡¡ This paper explores the role of input and output in second language acquisition £¨SLA £© by e m ploying Lev S £® Vygotsky ¬ð s £¨1896 £-1934 £© socioculturaltheory £® Krashen ¬ð sinput hypothesis £¨1982 £¬ 1985 £© and Swain ¬ð s output hypothesis £¨1985 £¬ 1993 £© hold different and so mew hat contradictory view points on the roles of input and output in SLA £® The difference might be solved by applying Vygotsky ¬ð s sociocultural theory £¬ w hich claims hu man higher mental functioning is constructed in a social £¬ cultural £¬ historical £¬ and institutional context £® This context is a web w oven by social interactions £¬ a dialectic unity of input and output £® Therefore £¬ according to Vygotsky ¬ð s approach to the understanding of learning £¬ the interactions between input and output give rise to second language develop ment £® Key w ords sociocultural theory £» input £» output £» second language acquisition 1 £® Introduction Second language researchers have not reached an agree m ent about the roles of input and output in second language acquisition £¨SLA £© £¬ although both have been widely studied respectively and separately £¨Krashen 1989 £» Sole 1994 £» Constantino 1995 £» Krashen 1997 £» Swain £¦ Lapkin 1995 £» Izu mi et al £® 1999 £» Izu mi £¦ Bigelow 2000 £» Feng £¦ Huang 2004 £© £® Studies on the role of input m ainly used Krashen ¬ð s input hypothesis £¨1982 £¬ 1985 £© as their theoretical fra m ew orks £¬ w hereas studies on that of output typically based their research on Swain ¬ð s output hypothesis £¨1985 £¬ 1993 £© £® These tw o hypotheses £¬ however £¬ hold different view points about the roles of input and outputin SLA £® Krashen £¨1982 £¬ 1985 £© claim ed that only co m prehensible input causes language acquisition £¬ w hile Swain £¨1985 £¬ 1993 £© proposed that through output £¬ either speaking or writing £¬ language acquisition might occur too £® Their discrepant views aboutthe roles of input and output leave language teachers and learners in a dile m m a £® Should teachers m aximize co m prehensible input by providing students with lectures £¬ reading progra ms £¬ and listening opportunities £¬ as Krashen suggested £¬ or to arrange considerable speaking and writing opportunities for students to practice as Swain im plied £¿ The tim e in class or the tim e for learning a new language is limited £¬ and m ore tim e and effort on input m eans less tim e on output £® So m e researchers claim that both input and output are essentialin language learning £® However £¬ their claim cannot find support fro m relevant SLA theories £® If they cite Krashen ¬ð s £¨1982 £¬ 1985 £© point to support the m £¬ they cannot use Swain ¬ð s output hypothesis £¨1985 £¬ 1993 £© £¬ because £¬ logically £¬ one cannot believe that output does not cause acquisition and output leads to acquisition at the sa m e tim e £® This paper tentatively applies the sociocultural theory of Vygotsky £¨1896 £-1934 £© £¬ one of the m ost influential philosophers and psychologists in the 20th century and a representative of social constructivism £¬ to explore the roles of input and output in SLA £® As an inter £-discipline £¬ SLA has constantly been nourished by theories in the fields of philosophy £¬ psychology £¬ and sociology £¬ as well as linguistics £® 7 8

Related Papers

Http Dx Doi Org 10 1080 01443410 2013 814198

Zohreh Eslami

interaction hypothesis and sociocultural theory

Saeid Najafi Sarem

The realm of language acquisition, either first or second language has been under the influence of three major theories, namely Behaviorism, Innativism, and Interactionism. The key figures in these schools of thought are Skinner, Chomsky, and Vygotsky respectively. Each theory has contributed to the field by highlighting a specific aspect of the language acquisition process. Behaviorist theory has given the main role to the environment, introducing the concepts of imitation and habit-formation. On the other hand, the innativist theory has focused on the role of mind and cognitive processes in language learning. Taking advantage of both the behaviorist and innativist theories, in the 19th century ,the interactinist approach emerged which concentrated on the role of social interaction in language learning. Based on this approach learners should be exposed to comprehensible, negotiated, or modified input in their attempts to acquire a language. In the same line, the present article tries to provide a critical literature on the interactionist approach in second language learning. Therefore, this review first sheds light on the major theoretical points introduced by this theory; then it tries to discuss some of the main implications and applications of the social interactionist approach in the domain of second language learning. Keywords: constructivism, interactionist approach, interaction hypothesis

Language Teaching

Tracy G. Beckett

The intent of this chapter is to familiarize readers with the principles and constructs of an approach to learning and mental development known as Socio-cultural Theory. 1 Sociocultural Theory (SCT) has its origins in the writings of the Russian psychologist L. S. Vygotsky and his colleagues. SCT argues that human mental functioning is fundamentally a mediated process that is organized by cultural artifacts, activities, and concepts (Ratner, 2002). 2 Within this framework, humans are understood to utilize existing cultural artifacts and to create new ones that allow them to regulate their biological and be-havioral activity. Language use, organization, and structure are the primary means of mediation. Practically speaking, developmental processes take place through participation in cultural, linguistic, and historically formed settings such as family life and peer group interaction, and in institutional contexts like schooling, organized sports activities, and work places, to name only a few. SCT argues that while human neurobiology is a necessary condition for higher order thinking, the most important forms of human cognitive activity develop 197 1 Recently, some scholars working in this theory have begun to use the term Cultural-Historical Activity Theory, or CHAT. However, most research conducted on L2 learning within this general theoretical framework has used the term sociocultural, and for this reason we will use this term throughout the chapter. We also think it is important to point out that the term sociocultural has been used by researchers who do not work directly within the theoretical perspective we are addressing in this chapter. These researchers use the terms sociocultural to refer to the general social and cultural circumstances in which individuals conduct the business of living.

Azizullah Mirzaei

Steven L Thorne

The intent of this chapter is to familiarize readers with the principles and constructs of an approach to learning and mental development known as sociocultural theory. 1 Sociocultural theory (hereafter SCT) has its origins in the writings of the Russian psychologist L. S. Vygotsky and his colleagues. SCT argues that human mental functioning is fundamentally a mediated process that is organized by cultural artifacts, activities, and concepts (Ratner, 2002). 2 Within this framework, humans are understood to utilize existing, and to create new, cultural artifacts that allow them to regulate, or more fully monitor and control, their behavior. Practically speaking, developmental processes take place through participation in cultural, linguistic, and historically formed settings such as family life, peer group interaction, and in institutional contexts like schooling, organized social activities, and work places (to name only a few). SCT argues that while human neurobiology is a necessary condition for higher mental processes, the 1 Scholars working in Vygotsky-inspired frameworks also use the term Cultural-Historical Activity Theory (or CHAT). However, most research conducted on L2 learning within the Vygotskian tradition has used the term sociocultural and for this reason, we will use this term throughout the chapter. It is also important to note that the term sociocultural has been used by researchers who do not work directly within the theoretical perspective we are addressing in this chapter. These researchers use the term sociocultural to refer to the general social and cultural circumstances in which individuals conduct the business of living. 2 In this chapter, we restrict our discussion to symbolic artifacts, in particular language. For a more complete discussion of mediation, see Lantolf and Thorne (2006).

Steven L Thorne , John Hellermann

Leticia Vicente-Rasoamalala

Reza Norouzian

RELATED PAPERS

Kristin Davin , Paul D Toth

Foreign Language Annals

Francis John Troyan , Kristin Davin

Parviz Maftoon

Abdelhafid Smaili

Amir Hassanein

Science Park Research Organization & Counselling

Zehra Gabillon

Soohyun Han

Rachel Hawkes

Breffni O'Rourke

Journal of Language Teaching

Holbrook Mahn

Rieko Nishida , Tomoko Yashima

Ratikorn Sirisatit

Celeste Kinginger

The Open University UK

Steve Graham

Farid Saydee, Ph.D.

Journal of Language Teaching …

Hamdollah Ravand

Educazione Linguistica. Language Education EL.LE

melisa Perea

Ilona Vandergriff

Dr. Prudhvi Raju

Long V Nguyen

Margherita (Maggie) Berti

Zahra Karimi

dayvid junio

Eun-Young Jang

Muhammad Thahir

Tim Murphey

Melinda Martin-Beltran

Caroline Payant

Current pathways in foreign language teaching

Seher Balbay

About Language Learning & Technology

Jeff McQuillan

Parvin Safari

Sense Publishers

Caroline Bligh

Mark Darhower

Vera Gonzales

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

Language Humanities

Advertiser Disclosure

How we make money, what is the interaction hypothesis, our promise to you.

Founded in 2002, our company has been a trusted resource for readers seeking informative and engaging content. Our dedication to quality remains unwavering—and will never change. We follow a strict editorial policy , ensuring that our content is authored by highly qualified professionals and edited by subject matter experts. This guarantees that everything we publish is objective, accurate, and trustworthy.

Over the years, we've refined our approach to cover a wide range of topics, providing readers with reliable and practical advice to enhance their knowledge and skills. That's why millions of readers turn to us each year. Join us in celebrating the joy of learning, guided by standards you can trust.

Editorial Standards

At LanguageHumanities, we are committed to creating content that you can trust. Our editorial process is designed to ensure that every piece of content we publish is accurate, reliable, and informative.

Our team of experienced writers and editors follows a strict set of guidelines to ensure the highest quality content. We conduct thorough research, fact-check all information, and rely on credible sources to back up our claims. Our content is reviewed by subject matter experts to ensure accuracy and clarity.

We believe in transparency and maintain editorial independence from our advertisers. Our team does not receive direct compensation from advertisers, allowing us to create unbiased content that prioritizes your interests.

The Interaction Hypothesis is a type of theory proposing that one of most effective methods of learning a new language is through personal and direct interaction. This theory is applied specifically to the acquisition of a foreign or a second language . It is usually attributed to Professor Michael Long, when he wrote a paper entitled “The Role of the Linguistic Environment in Second Language Acquisition ” in 1996.

Through the Interaction Hypothesis, Professor Long integrated and reconciled two hypotheses on second language acquisition (SLA): the input and the output hypotheses. The Input Hypothesis states that a language learner only needs to be supplied with “input” through the forms of reading, listening to conversations, and lessons on grammar and vocabulary. The Output Hypothesis, on the other hand, stresses the importance of practicing and speaking to retain and remember the language. The Interaction Hypothesis combines both the “input” and “output” by stating that interaction is not only a means for a learner to study the language, but also a way for the learner to practice what he has learned.

interaction hypothesis and sociocultural theory

Among the types of interactions, conversation is probably the most emphasized in the Interaction Hypothesis, an idea most probably derived from the “ discourse approach” by Professor Evelyn Hatch who, in 1978, wrote papers that stressed the importance of constant communication and interaction for SLA. The Interaction Hypothesis acknowledges that during conversations, there are certain situations wherein a participant does not understand what the other says, but it is in these situations where learning becomes more effective. The theory refers to this occurrence as “negotiation,” wherein the participants will attempt to understand and repair the miscommunication during the interaction.

The first step in the negotiation is the interaction itself, when both participants begin to engage in conversation. The second step, the “negative feedback,” occurs when a participant does not understand a certain word, sometimes seen in a nonverbal action such as in the furrowing of the brow. In some cases, the other participant may request clarification by saying, “Pardon?” or “Can you say that again?” The process wherein the misunderstood participant strives to make the other participant understand is called “modification output.” The participant may paraphrase or give examples to make the meaning of the word clearer, until the other participant responds in an affirmative way that he has understood.

Interaction Hypothesis suggests an interaction between a second-language learner and a native speaker, so the learner can study the language in its most authentic setting. In this way, the learner not only learns about the language, but also the nuances and other nonverbal cues the go along with the words. Many universities in English-speaking countries have English programs and classes focusing on personal interaction for many foreign students who go abroad just to learn how to speak English.

Related Articles

  • What Is Conversation Theory?
  • What Is the Input Hypothesis?
  • What Are the Different Types of Second Language Acquisition Theories?
  • What is the Social Exchange Theory?

Discussion Comments

Languagehumanities, in your inbox.

Our latest articles, guides, and more, delivered daily.

Proceedings of the International Conference on Education, Language, Art and Intercultural Communication

Vocational College English Teaching Inspiration from Sociocultural Theory and Interaction Hypothesis

This article will present the situation of college English teaching in higher vocational colleges, and introduce two latest second language acquisition theories which are "social cultural theory" and "interaction hypothesis". The aim of this article is to explore the new modes of college English teaching in order to effectively improve the higher vocational students' ability of using language and intercultural communication ability.

Download article (PDF)

Cite this article

Sociocultural Theory

  • Living reference work entry
  • First Online: 17 April 2024
  • Cite this living reference work entry

interaction hypothesis and sociocultural theory

  • Liu Qinxue 2  

Sociocultural Theory, also known as cultural-historical psychology, studies human’s mental development from a sociocultural perspective. Between the mid-1920s and the early 1930s, influenced by the German thinker and philosopher Friedrich Engels’ thought about the role of labor in the process of human adapting to and transforming nature, the Soviet psychologist Lev Vygotsky elaborated the basic principles and ways by which human mental functions develop, and proposed the sociocultural theory.

Vygotsky categorizes mental functions into lower and higher and posits that an individual’s mental development involves transitioning from lower to higher mental functions, influenced by their environment and education. Reasons underlying the transition are as follows: (1) Mental functions are shaped by the development of society, culture, and history and follow social laws. (2) Through communications with adults, children acquire languages and symbols, which are mediational tools for the...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

Further Reading

Shaffer DR, Kipp K (2020) Developmental psychology: childhood and adolescence, 9th edn. Cengage Learning, Boston

Google Scholar  

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

School of Psychology, Central China Normal University, Wuhan, China

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Liu Qinxue .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2024 The Editor(s) (if applicable) and The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this entry

Cite this entry.

Qinxue, L. (2024). Sociocultural Theory. In: The ECPH Encyclopedia of Psychology. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-99-6000-2_851-1

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-99-6000-2_851-1

Received : 23 March 2024

Accepted : 25 March 2024

Published : 17 April 2024

Publisher Name : Springer, Singapore

Print ISBN : 978-981-99-6000-2

Online ISBN : 978-981-99-6000-2

eBook Packages : Springer Reference Behavioral Science and Psychology Reference Module Humanities and Social Sciences Reference Module Business, Economics and Social Sciences

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

IMAGES

  1. 18 Sociocultural Theory Examples (2024)

    interaction hypothesis and sociocultural theory

  2. Vygotsky's Sociocultural Theory Diagram

    interaction hypothesis and sociocultural theory

  3. Sociocultural Theory: Understanding Vygotsky's Theory

    interaction hypothesis and sociocultural theory

  4. PPT

    interaction hypothesis and sociocultural theory

  5. Lev Vygotsky Sociocultural Theory

    interaction hypothesis and sociocultural theory

  6. MAT Program's View of Sociocultural Theory

    interaction hypothesis and sociocultural theory

VIDEO

  1. Theory Application Assignment: Behaviorism and Sociocultural Theory

  2. Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Theory of Cognitive Development

  3. Sociological Theories

  4. Unlocking the Supernatural Exploring the Paranormal Interaction Hypothesis

  5. Sociological Theories

  6. Interaction Hypothesis

COMMENTS

  1. PDF The interaction hypothesis: A literature review

    explanation. The interaction hypothesis (IH) is an excellent example. The IH, which has also been referred to as the input, interaction, and output model by Block (2003), the interaction theory by Carroll (1999), the oral interaction hypothesis by Ellis (1991), and the interaction approach by Gass and Mackey (2007), was first proposed by Long ...

  2. Vygotsky's Sociocultural Theory Of Cognitive Development

    Lev Vygotsky's theory of child development, known as the sociocultural theory, emphasizes the importance of social interaction and cultural context in learning and cognitive development. Vygotsky proposed the concept of the Zone of Proximal Development (ZPD), which is the gap between what a child can do independently and what they can achieve with guidance from a more knowledgeable person.

  3. Theories of second-language acquisition

    Interaction hypothesis Long's ... Sociocultural theory is the notion that human mental function is from participating cultural mediation integrated into social activities. The central thread of sociocultural theory focuses on diverse social, historical, cultural, ...

  4. (PDF) SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION: RECONCILING THEORIES

    comprehension hypothesis, interaction hypothesis, output h ypothesis, sociocultural theory, and connectionism. These, in my opinion, are the ones that have had the greatest influence on the field.

  5. Interactionist Perspectives on Second Language Acquisition

    11 Sociocultural Theory and the Pedagogical ... field of SLA, focusing on a number of different areas, including language transfer, language universals, and input and interaction. She is the author of Input, Interaction, and the Second Language Learner, coauthor with Larry Selinker of Second Language Acquisition: An Introductory Course, and ...

  6. (PDF) A Critical Review of the Interactionist Approach to Second

    Ellis (1999) claims that in spite of the fact that socio-cultural paradigm has a lot to offer how interaction leads to acquisition and constructs meaning, it has some weaknesses. One is that the followers of this theory have examined L2 use rather than L2 acquisition. There is no distinction between use and acquisition in socio -cultural ...

  7. Interaction hypothesis

    The interaction hypothesis is a theory of second-language acquisition which states that the development of language proficiency is promoted by face-to-face interaction and communication. Its main focus is on the role of input, interaction, and output in second language acquisition. It posits that the level of language that a learner is exposed to must be such that the learner is able to ...

  8. PDF SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION: RECONCILING THEORIES

    comprehension hypothesis, interaction hypothesis, output hypothesis, sociocultural theory, and connectionism. These, in my opinion, are the ones that have had the greatest influence on the field.

  9. Interaction Hypothesis: A comprehensive theory of SLA?

    Interaction Hypothesis (IH) introduced by Long accepts the role of "nurture" ... Finally, Ellis's Interactionist Theory, a combination of IH and socio-cultural theory, is suggested as an updated version of IH. Key words: Interaction Hypothesis, Second Language Acquisition, SLA theory ELT VOICES - INDIA August 2014 | Volume 4, Issue 4 1. ...

  10. Interaction Theory of Language Development

    The interaction theory recognizes that both environmental and biological factors are important in language development [2]. Within this theory of compromise are theorists who are closer to one end of the extreme than the other. However, all interactionists believe that language acquisition occurs as a result of the natural interaction between ...

  11. Vygotsky and Sociocultural Approaches to Teaching and Learning

    University of New Mexico, Department of Language, Literacy & Sociocultural Studies, Albuquerque, NM, USA. Search for more papers by this author. ... the internal system of meaning that is created through the use of language in social interaction and that is central to concept formation. Having described Vygotsky's theory and methodology, we ...

  12. Learning a second language through interaction

    The principal perspectives are those afforded by the Interaction Hypothesis, Socio-Cultural Theory and the Levels of Processing model. Interaction is, therefore, defined broadly; it is seen as involving both intermental and intramental activity. The theoretical perspectives are explored empirically in a series of studies which investigate the ...

  13. [PDF] The psycholinguistics of the interaction approach

    This book examines different theoretical perspectives on the role that interaction plays in second language acquisition, including those afforded by the Interaction Hypothesis, Socio-Cultural Theory and the Levels of Processing model. Expand

  14. INTRODUCTION: The Role of Oral and Written Corrective Feedback ...

    psycholinguistic theories of SLA such as the interaction hypothesis (Long, 1991, 1996), the output hypothesis (Swain, 1985, 1995), the ... According to sociocultural theory, the effec-tiveness of CF lies in its propensity for scaffolding interaction to construct a zone of proximal development (where learners are assisted ...

  15. Interaction Hypothesis: A Comprehensive Theory

    The role of interaction in second language acquisition has always been controversial. A bunch of theories have been proposed as to the role of "nature" or "nurture" in SLA. Interaction Hypothesis (IH) introduced by Long accepts the role of "nurture" in SLA claiming that negotiation of meaning through interaction can facilitate the process of language learning. In this review, the ...

  16. Sociocultural Theory: Understanding Vygotsky's Theory

    History of Sociocultural Theory . Sociocultural theory grew from the work of psychologist Lev Vygotsky, who believed that parents, caregivers, peers, and the culture at large are responsible for developing higher-order functions. According to Vygotsky, learning has its basis in interacting with other people.

  17. (Pdf) Vygotsky ¬Ð Ssociocultural Theory and The Role of Input and

    ¡¡¡¡ This paper explores the role of input and output in second language acquisition £¨SLA £© by e m ploying Lev S £® Vygotsky ¬ð s £¨1896 £-1934 £© socioculturaltheory £® Krashen ¬ð sinput hypothesis £¨1982 £¬ 1985 £© and Swain ¬ð s output hypothesis £¨1985 £¬ 1993 £© hold different and so mew hat contradictory view points on the roles of input and output in ...

  18. Interaction, Output, and Communicative Language Learning

    Summary This chapter contains section titled: Introduction Whether Interaction Promotes L2 Learning How Interaction Facilitates L2 Learning Pedagogical Implications for Communicative Language Learning

  19. PDF Vocational College English Teaching

    sociocultural theory is: the senior cognitive function of human being could make use of the product of social culture ——symbols to mediate the relationship between individual

  20. What Is the Interaction Hypothesis?

    The Interaction Hypothesis is a type of theory proposing that one of most effective methods of learning a new language is through personal and direct interaction. This theory is applied specifically to the acquisition of a foreign or a second language.It is usually attributed to Professor Michael Long, when he wrote a paper entitled "The Role of the Linguistic Environment in Second Language ...

  21. [PDF] An Investigation of Social Interaction in the Second Language

    Long (1983) and Vygotsky (1987) have investigated the learning process of second language learners and argued that second language learning can happen through inclass interaction and oral communication. According to Long's (1983) Interaction Hypothesis theory, the interactional collaboration among peers can lead to second language learning. Even more, second language learners are more likely ...

  22. Vocational College English Teaching Inspiration from Sociocultural

    Sociocultural Theory, Interaction Hypothesis, College English teaching Abstract. This article will present the situation of college English teaching in higher vocational colleges, and introduce two latest second language acquisition theories which are "social cultural theory" and "interaction hypothesis". The aim of this article is to explore ...

  23. Sociocultural Theory

    Sociocultural Theory, also known as cultural-historical psychology, studies human's mental development from a sociocultural perspective. Between the mid-1920s and the early 1930s, influenced by the German thinker and philosopher Friedrich Engels' thought about the role of labor in the process of human adapting to and transforming nature, the Soviet psychologist Lev Vygotsky elaborated the ...

  24. [PDF] An Introduction to Behaviourism, Innatism and Interactionism

    There are many theories that have influence on the field of second language acquisition, and all of the theories have different principles and ideas. The knowledge about theories, methods, approaches, strategies and techniques is essential in order to have effective language classes. However, many of the instructors and teachers have lack of information about the language theories based on a ...